Amlung, M., Marsden, E., Holshausen, K., Morris, V., Patel, H., Vedelago, L., . . .McCabe, R. E. (2019). Delay discounting as a transdiagnostic process in psychiatric disorders: A meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry, 76(11), 1176–1186.
https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.2102 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Arnold, K. M., McDermott, K. B., Szpunar, K. K. (2011). Imagining the near and far future: The role of location familiarity. Memory & Cognition, 39(6), 954–967.
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-011-0076-1 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Athamneh, L. N., Stein, M. D., Lin, E. H., Stein, J. S., Mellis, A. M., Gatchalian, K. M., . . .Bickel, W. K. (2020). Setting a goal could help you control: Comparing the effect of health goal versus general episodic future thinking on health behaviors among cigarette smokers and obese individuals. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 29(1), 59–72.
https://doi.org/10.1037/pha0000351 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Baumeister, R. F. (2014). Self-regulation, ego depletion, and inhibition. Neuropsychologia, 65, 313–319.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.08.012 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Benoit, R. G., Gilbert, S. J., Burgess, P. W. (2011). A neural mechanism mediating the impact of episodic prospection on farsighted decisions. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(18), 6771–6779.
https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.6559-10.2011 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Białaszek, W., Ostaszewski, P. (2012). Discounting of sequences of delayed rewards of different amounts. Behavioural Processes, 89(1), 39–43.
https://doi.org/org/10.1016/j.beproc.2011.10.014 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Bobova, L., Finn, P. R., Rickert, M. E., Lucas, J. (2009). Disinhibitory psychopathology and delay discounting in alcohol dependence: Personality and cognitive correlates. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 17(1), 51–61.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014503 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P., Rothstein, H. R. (2005). Comprehensive meta-analysis (Version 2). Englewood Cliffs.
Google Scholar
Boyer, P. (2008). Evolutionary economics of mental time travel? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(6), 219–224.
https://doi.org/org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.03.003 Google Scholar
Brocas, I., Carrillo, J. D. (2018). A neuroeconomic theory of mental time travel. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 12, Article 658.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2018.00658 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Bromberg, U., Lobatcheva, M., Peters, J. (2017). Episodic future thinking reduces temporal discounting in healthy adolescents. PLOS ONE, 12(11), Article e0188079.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0188079 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Bromberg, U., Wiehler, A., Peters, J. (2015). Episodic future thinking is related to impulsive decision making in healthy adolescents. Child Development, 86(5), 1458–1468.
https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12390 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Bulley, A., Gullo, M. J. (2017). The influence of episodic foresight on delay discounting and demand for alcohol. Addictive Behaviors, 66, 1–6.
https://doi.org/org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2016.11.003 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Bulley, A., Miloyan, B., Pepper, G. V., Gullo, M. J., Henry, J. D., Suddendorf, T. (2019). Cuing both positive and negative episodic foresight reduces delay discounting but does not affect risk-taking. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72(8), 1998–2017.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1747021818819777 Google Scholar |
SAGE Journals
Bulley, A., Schacter, D. L. (2020). Deliberating trade-offs with the future. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(3), 238–247.
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-0834-9 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Calluso, C., Tosoni, A., Cannito, L., Committeri, G. (2019). Concreteness and emotional valence of episodic future thinking (EFT) independently affect the dynamics of intertemporal decisions. PLOS ONE, 14(5), Article e0217224.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0217224 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Carr, K. A., Hollis-Hansen, K., Austin, K., Epstein, L. H. (2021). Written or drawn episodic future thinking cues improves delay discounting in adults. Learning and Motivation, 74, 101727.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lmot.2021.101727 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Cheng, Y. Y., Shein, P. P., Chiou, W. B. (2012). Escaping the impulse to immediate gratification: The prospect concept promotes a future-oriented mindset, prompting an inclination towards delayed gratification. British Journal of Psychology, 103(1), 129–141.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.2011.02067.x Google Scholar |
Crossref
Chiou, W. B., Wu, W. H. (2017). Episodic future thinking involving the nonsmoking self can induce lower discounting and cigarette consumption. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 78(1), 106–112.
https://doi.org/10.15288/jsad.2017.78.106 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Ciaramelli, E., De Luca, F., Kwan, D., Mok, J., Bianconi, F., Knyagnytska, V., . . .Rosenbaum, R. S. (2021). The role of ventromedial prefrontal cortex in reward valuation and future thinking during intertemporal choice. eLife, 10, e67387.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.67387 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Ciaramelli, E., Sellitto, M., Tosarelli, G., di Pellegrino, G. (2019). Imagining Events Alternative to the Present Can Attenuate Delay Discounting. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 13, 269.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00269 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Craft, W. H., Tegge, A. N., Bickel, W. K. (2020). Episodic future thinking reduces chronic pain severity: A proof of concept study. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 215, 108250.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2020.108250 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Daniel, T. O., Said, M., Stanton, C. M., Epstein, L. H. (2015). Episodic future thinking reduces delay discounting and energy intake in children. Eating Behaviors, 18, 20–24.
https://doi.org/org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2015.03.006 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Daniel, T. O., Sawyer, A., Dong, Y. L., Bickel, W. K., Epstein, L. H. (2016). Remembering versus imagining: When does episodic retrospection and episodic prospection aid decision making? Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 5(3), 352–358.
https://doi.org/org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2016.06.005 Google Scholar
Daniel, T. O., Stanton, C. M., Epstein, L. H. (2013a). The future is now: Comparing the effect of episodic future thinking on impulsivity in lean and obese individuals. Appetite, 71, 120–125.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2013.07.010 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Daniel, T. O., Stanton, C. M., Epstein, L. H. (2013b). The future is now: Reducing impulsivity and energy intake using episodic future thinking. Psychological Science, 24(11), 2339–2342.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613488780 Google Scholar |
SAGE Journals
D’Argembeau, A., Xue, G., Lu, Z. L., Van der Linden, M., Bechara, A. (2008). Neural correlates of envisioning emotional events in the near and far future. NeuroImage, 40(1), 398–407.
https://doi.org/org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.11.025 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Dassen, F. C. M., Jansen, A., Nederkoorn, C., Houben, K. (2016). Focus on the future: Episodic future thinking reduces discount rate and snacking. Appetite, 96, 327–332.
https://doi.org/org/10.1016/j.appet.2015.09.032 Google Scholar
Figner, B., Knoch, D., Johnson, E. J., Krosch, A. R., Lisanby, S. H., Fehr, E., Weber, E. U. (2010). Lateral prefrontal cortex and self-control in intertemporal choice. Nature Neuroscience, 13(5), 538–539.
https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2516 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Frederick, S., Loewenstein, G., O’Donoghue, T. (2002). Time discounting and time preference: A critical review. Journal of Economic Literature, 40(2), 351–401.
https://doi.org/10.1257/002205102320161311 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Giles, E. L., Robalino, S., McColl, E., Sniehotta, F. F., Adams, J. (2014). The effectiveness of financial incentives for health behaviour change: Systematic review and meta-analysis. PLOS ONE, 9(3), Article e90347.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0090347 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Green, L., Myerson, J. (2004). A discounting framework for choice with delayed and probabilistic rewards. Psychological Bulletin, 130(5), 769–792.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.130.5.769 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Hamilton, K. R., Mitchell, M. R., Wing, V. C., Balodis, I. M., Bickel, W. K., Fillmore, M., . . .Moeller, F. G. (2015). Choice impulsivity: Definitions, measurement issues, and clinical implications. Personality Disorders: Theory Research and Treatment, 6(2), 182–198.
https://doi.org/10.1037/per0000099 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Hare, T. A., Camerer, C. F., Rangel, A. (2009). Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system. Science, 324(5927), 646–648.
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1168450 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Hedges, L. V., Vevea, J. L. (1998). Fixed- and random-effects models in meta-analysis. Psychological Methods, 3(4), 486–504.
https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989x.3.4.486 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Hendrickson, K. L., Rasmussen, E. B. (2017). Mindful eating reduces impulsive food choice in adolescents and adults. Health Psychology, 36(3), 226–235.
https://doi.org/10.1037/hea0000440 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Hollis-Hansen, K., O’Donnell, S. E., Seidman, J. S., Brande, S. J., Epstein, L. H. (2019). Improvements in episodic future thinking methodology: Establishing a standardized episodic thinking control. PLOS ONE, 14(3), Article e0214397.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0214397 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Hu, X. C., Kleinschmidt, H., Martin, J. A., Han, Y., Thelen, M., Meiberth, D., . . .Weber, B. (2017). A reduction in delay discounting by using episodic future imagination and the association with episodic memory capacity. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, Article 663.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00663 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Hu, X. C., Uhle, F., Fliessbach, K., Wagner, M., Han, Y., Weber, B., Jessen, F. (2017). Reduced future-oriented decision making in individuals with subjective cognitive decline: A functional MRI study. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 6, 222–231.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dadm.2017.02.005 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Jenkins, A. C., Hsu, M. (2017). Dissociable contributions of imagination and willpower to the malleability of human patience. Psychological Science, 28(7), 894–906.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617698133 Google Scholar |
SAGE Journals
Jia, L. X., Liu, Z., Cui, J. F., Ding, Q. Y., Ye, J. Y., Liu, L. L., . . .Wang, Y. (2020). Future thinking is related to lower delay discounting than recent thinking, regardless of the magnitude of the reward, in individuals with schizotypy. Australian Psychologist, 55, 572–581.
https://doi.org/org/10.1111/ap.12460 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Kim, H., Schnall, S., White, M. P. (2013). Similar psychological distance reduces temporal discounting. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39(8), 1005–1016.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167213488214 Google Scholar |
SAGE Journals
Lawyer, S. R., Schoepflin, F., Green, R., Jenks, C. (2011). Discounting of hypothetical and potentially real outcomes in nicotine-dependent and nondependent samples. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 19(4), 263–274.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024141 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Lempert, K. M., Phelps, E. A. (2016). The malleability of intertemporal choice. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(1), 64–74.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.09.005 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Lempert, K. M., Speer, M. E., Delgado, M. R., Phelps, E. A. (2017). Positive autobiographical memory retrieval reduces temporal discounting. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(10), 1584–1593.
https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsx086 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Lempert, K. M., Steinglass, J. E., Pinto, A., Kable, J. W., Simpson, H. B. (2019). Can delay discounting deliver on the promise of RDoC? Psychological Medicine, 49(2), 190–199.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291718001770 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Lin, H., Epstein, L. H. (2014). Living in the moment: Effects of time perspective and emotional valence of episodic thinking on delay discounting. Behavioral Neuroscience, 128(1), 12–19.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035705 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Liu, L., Feng, T. Y., Chen, J., Li, H. (2013). The value of emotion: How does episodic prospection modulate delay discounting? PLOS ONE, 8(11), Article e81717.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0081717 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Logue, A. W. (1988). Research on self-control: An integrating framework. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11(4), 665–678.
https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x00053978 Google Scholar |
Crossref
MacKillop, J., Amlung, M. T., Few, L. R., Ray, L. A., Sweet, L. H., Munafo, M. R. (2011). Delayed reward discounting and addictive behavior: A meta-analysis. Psychopharmacology, 216(3), 305–321.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-011-2229-0 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Madden, G. J., Begotka, A. M., Raiff, B. R., Kastern, L. L. (2003). Delay discounting of real and hypothetical rewards. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 11(2), 139–145.
https://doi.org/10.1037/1064-1297.11.2.139 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Madden, G. J., Raiff, B. R., Lagorio, C. H., Begotka, A. M., Mueller, A. M., Hehli, D. J., Wegener, A. A. (2004). Delay discounting of potentially real and hypothetical rewards: II. Between- and within-subject comparisons. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 12(4), 251–261.
https://doi.org/10.1037/1064-1297.12.4.251 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Mansouri, T. H., Crandall, A. K., Temple, J. L. (2021). The effect of repeated episodic future thinking on the relative reinforcing value of snack food. Journal of Health Psychology, 26(13), 2402–2413.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1359105320914060 Google Scholar |
SAGE Journals
Miloyan, B., Bulley, A., Suddendorf, T. (2016). Episodic foresight and anxiety: Proximate and ultimate perspectives. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 55(1), 4–22.
https://doi.org/10.1111/bjc.12080 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., Altman, D. G. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. BMJ, 339, Article b2535.
https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.b2535 Google Scholar |
Crossref
Mok, J. N. Y., Kwan, D., Green, L., Myerson, J., Craver, C. F., Rosenbaum, R. S. (
留言 (0)