Effects of cannabinoid exposure on short-term memory and medial orbitofrontal cortex function and chemistry in adolescent female rhesus macaques

Introduction

The use of cannabis and other cannabinergic drugs during adolescence, a developmental period when the brain undergoes maturation, is widespread and continues to grow (Bava et al., 2010; Peters et al., 2022). It is uncertain whether cannabinoid exposure during this time may lead to long-term alterations in cognition and/or brain function (Becker and Hu, 2008); however, a number of studies suggest that repeated exposure can adversely affect cognitive performance (Egerton et al., 2006; Crean et al., 2011; Zanettini et al., 2011). Of special concern, such deleterious effects on cognitive performance have been noted in a range of tasks involving fronto-cortical brain regions, which undergo extensive maturation throughout adolescence (Delevich et al., 2021). For example, deficits in the performance of cannabis users relative to age-matched controls have been observed in tasks of inhibitory control and cognitive flexibility, sustained attention, impulsivity, and memory (Gruber and Yurgelun-Todd, 2005; Fontes et al., 2011; Hanson et al., 2011; Tait et al., 2011; Wrege et al., 2014). Thus, considerable evidence supports the idea that cannabis use during adolescence can have a persistent negative impact on cognitive function.

Disruptions in cognitive performance, especially those related to memory function, previously have been observed in preclinical studies of Δ9-tetrahydrocannabinol (Δ9-THC) and other cannabinergic drugs in rodents and non-human primates. For example, daily cannabinoid exposure was shown to impair novel object recognition, short-term memory, and cognitive flexibility in adolescent rats (O’Shea et al., 2004, 2006; Cha et al., 2007; Quinn et al., 2008; Higuera-Matas et al., 2009; Zamberletti et al., 2014). Similarly, spatial memory in adolescent monkeys also was disrupted by daily Δ9-THC exposure over the course of 6 months (Verrico et al., 2014).

Neuroimaging studies in human subjects have provided insight into the brain regions that underlie different types of task performance (Huettel, 2012) as well as those that may be altered in adolescent cannabis users (Sager and Gruber, 2019). For example, studies of resting-state functional connectivity (RSFC) suggest a relationship between extent of cannabis use and abnormal connectivity within fronto-temporal networks (Houck et al., 2013; Cheng et al., 2014; Camchong et al., 2017). Adolescent cannabis use, in particular, has been associated with reduced interhemispheric RSFC and elevated right hemisphere connectivity that may underlie the disruption of coherent, organized cognitive and behavioral processes (Orr et al., 2013). Adolescent-onset cannabis users also may display altered patterns of brain activity (primarily in fronto-cortical regions) when it is measured during cognitive tasks, including short-term memory of several types (Padula et al., 2007; Tapert et al., 2007; Schweinsburg et al., 2008; Jacobus et al., 2009; Sagar et al., 2015). Although the results of fMRI experiments may vary depending on the particular methodology and regions of interest, the altered pattern of communication between brain areas at rest in cannabis users suggests that qualitative changes in intrinsic connectivity may be related to deficits in cognitive performance.

Research in human subjects to solidify our understanding of the consequences of adolescent exposure to cannabinoids on behavior and brain function faces considerable challenges. For example, due to ethical and practical constraints, such research (especially in adolescents) is predominantly cross-sectional rather than longitudinal. Moreover, it is difficult to control for histories that include the use or abuse of multiple substances as well as for other factors such as duration of use, age of initiation, frequency/amount of consumption, drugged versus withdrawal state (Churchwell et al., 2010; Lorenzetti et al., 2016), and polysubstance use (Moss et al., 2014). From this perspective, the extent to which results in human subjects reflect the effects of cannabis directly on behavior and brain function or, alternatively, contamination by such uncontrolled factors remains uncertain (Cheetham et al., 2012; Lubman et al., 2015). One approach to mitigating the problem of uncontrolled historical or contextual factors in preclinical research is through translational research in laboratory animals and, in particular, non-human primates. The neurological, phylogenetic, pharmacokinetic, and pharmacodynamic aspects of drug action in rhesus monkeys, for example, greatly overlap with those of humans (Weerts et al., 2007), permitting translationally-relevant longitudinal studies of the effects of cannabinoid exposure on complex behavior and brain function during adolescence.

In the present research, a touchscreen-based titrating-delay matching-to-sample task of short-term memory function (TDMTS; Kangas et al., 2010) was paired with longitudinal neuroimaging (fMRI and magnetic resonance spectroscopy) measures to investigate the effects of acute and chronic exposure to the cannabinoid receptor type 1 (CB1) agonist AM2389 in adolescent female rhesus macaques. Female macaques were studied in the present work because (1) cannabinergic sex differences have been documented, including a greater CB1 density across several brain regions in male than female laboratory animals (e.g., Rubino et al., 2008; Reich et al., 2009; Burston et al., 2010; Mateos et al., 2011), (2) the role of density in regions of interest related to memorial processes, including the orbitofrontal cortex (Lamar et al., 2004), are unknown, and (3) there continues to be a need to address the well-publicized underrepresentation of female subjects in biomedical research (Woitowich et al., 2020). Data also were obtained following chronic treatment to examine the immediate and longer-term effects of discontinuing daily injections of AM2389. Lastly, the effects of the CB1 antagonist rimonabant were determined during chronic AM2389 to assess the presence of behavioral or neural signs of antagonist-precipitated withdrawal. AM2389 was selected for study because its long duration of behavioral action (∼17 h in vivo half-life; Järbe et al., 2012) assures continuous CB1 receptor occupancy with once-daily treatment. In addition, as a CB1 receptor full agonist, its agonist effects may be more definitive than those of partial agonists like Δ9-THC (Paronis et al., 2012; Kangas et al., 2020). TDMTS task performance was selected as the cognitive endpoint for the present studies in view of the well-known ability of cannabinoids to disrupt short-term memory, an adverse effect that has been repeatedly documented in chronic cannabis users (Crean et al., 2011; Gruber et al., 2012; Crane et al., 2013) and preclinical studies in rodents and non-human primates (e.g., Cadoni et al., 2001; Verrico et al., 2012; Kangas et al., 2016). Neuroimaging procedures have focused on the medial orbitofrontal cortex, which has been shown to be involved in DMTS performance in an age-dependent manner (Lamar et al., 2004) and is also thought to play a critical role in substance abuse (London et al., 2000; Winstanley, 2007; Schoenbaum and Shaham, 2008).

Materials and methods Subjects

Four experimentally- and drug-naïve adolescent female rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) were studied in the present research. The subjects, which were 3.5–4.0 years old when studies were initiated, were singly housed in a temperature- and humidity-controlled vivarium with a 12-h light/dark cycle (lights on at 7 am). Each subject had continuous access to water in the home cage and was maintained at its approximate free-feeding weight by post-session feedings of a nutritionally balanced diet of fresh fruit, vegetables, and high protein biscuits (Purina Mills International, Brentwood, MO, United States); environmental enrichment (e.g., toys, puzzles, mirrors) was provided daily. The protocol for these studies was approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at McLean Hospital and was conducted in a facility licensed by the United States Department of Agriculture and in accordance with guidelines provided by the Committee on Care and Use of Laboratory Animals of the Institute of Laboratory Animals Resources, Commission on Life Sciences (National Research Council, 2010).

Apparatus

Details and schematics of the touchscreen chamber designed for rhesus macaques can be found in Kangas and Bergman (2017). Subjects were seated in non-human primate chairs (515-SASR, PLAS Labs, Inc., Lansing, MI, United States) within the chamber. Each chair was outfitted with a stainless-steel sipper tube that was readily accessible to the subject’s mouth for the delivery of liquid reinforcement [0.15 ml of 30% sweetened condensed milk dispensed via an infusion pump situated outside the experimental chamber (PHM-100-10, Med Associates, St. Albans, VT, United States)]. A 17” touch-sensitive screen (1739L, ELO TouchSystems, Menlo Park, CA, United States) was mounted to an inside wall of the enclosure directly in front of the subject and within easy reach. A speaker bar (NQ576AT, Hewlett-Packard, Palo Alto, CA, United States) was mounted above the touchscreen to provide audible feedback. All experimental events and data collection were programmed in E-Prime Professional 2.0 (Psychology Software Tools, Inc., Sharpsburg, PA, United States).

Experimental timeline

An experimental timeline of behavioral, drug administration, and imaging conditions is presented in Figure 1. As shown, dose-response data for the acute effects of AM2389 on short-term memory were obtained before and after chronic exposure to AM2389, with neuroimaging sessions to obtain MRS and fMRI data at several key time points. Scan sessions prior to initial dose-response determination of the behavioral effects of AM2389 provided baseline control data whereas scan sessions conducted on the first and last days of the chronic regimen provided data before and after the development of tolerance to the effects of AM2389 on short-term memory. Data obtained 30 days following discontinuation of the chronic treatment regimen offered insight into the persistence of neural changes observed during the chronic regimen.

FIGURE 1

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental timeline.

Titrating-delay matching-to-sample Training procedures

Subjects were first trained to respond on (i.e., touch) the touchscreen using previously described shaping procedures (Kangas and Bergman, 2012). After reliable responding was observed, subjects were next trained on a touchscreen-based matching-to-sample procedure (Kangas et al., 2016). Trials began with presentation of a 7 × 7 cm2 pictorial stimulus (either a photograph of green leaves or red cherries) presented in the center of the screen (sample stimulus). After completion of a fixed ratio (FR) 20 response requirement on the sample stimulus, the sample stimulus was replaced by presentation of both stimuli (comparison stimuli) positioned to the left and right of center. The position of the two stimuli—left or right—was assigned randomly from trial to trial. A single response on the comparison stimulus that matched the previously presented sample stimulus initiated the delivery of liquid reinforcement accompanied by an 880 ms yellow screen flash and followed by a 10 s intertrial interval (ITI) blackout. A response on the non-matching stimulus immediately initiated a 20 s ITI blackout. A correction procedure (Kangas and Branch, 2008) was implemented such that incorrect trials were repeated until a correct response was emitted. Sessions ended after the subject completed 48 correct trials. After a subject committed 5 or fewer errors in each of 5 consecutive sessions, it was exposed to the titrating-delay matching-to-sample (TDMTS) task. In this procedure, the contingencies remained the same as described above, with the exception that every 2 consecutive correct matches increased the delay value (retention interval) between sample stimulus offset and comparison stimuli onset by 1-s on the next trial; every incorrect response decreased the delay by 1 s on the next trial (Kangas et al., 2010). Each TDMTS session began with a 0 s delay and training sessions continued until session-wide average titrated delay values were ± 20% of the 5-session mean. Thereafter, drug testing began under identical conditions except that the session was terminated if 30 min elapsed without a response or after 2-hr total, whichever came first. Following the achievement of stable levels of responding per the above criteria, baseline MRI scans were conducted as described below.

Drug testing and dosing procedures Acute studies

The acute effects of AM2389 on short-term memory performance were determined by injecting vehicle or each of a range of doses (0.56–10.0 μg/kg) of AM2389 intramuscularly (i.m.) 3-hr prior to the TDMTS session. Doses were administered no more than once per week and were studied in a mixed order across subjects. After the completion of dose-response determinations with AM2389, task performance also was assessed 1 h following i.m. administration of the CB1 receptor antagonist rimonabant (1.0 mg/kg). These effects of rimonabant in untreated subjects provided control data for evaluating the effects of rimonabant administered during chronic AM2389 treatment.

Chronic treatment

After acute studies were completed, subjects were treated once daily in the morning (7 days/week) with AM2389. On the first day of the chronic regimen, subjects received 10 μg/kg, i.m. AM2389 3 h prior to a scanning session. This provided information regarding the acute MRS- and fMRI-related effects of the dose of AM2389 that substantially disrupted task performance in all subjects in preceding dose-response determinations and that was scheduled to be the terminal daily treatment dose in the ensuing chronic regimen. Starting the next day, TDMTS sessions were conducted 3 h after the injection, 5 days/week (Mon-Fri). The treatment dose of AM2389 began at 0.32 μg/kg/day and, in each subject, increased in 0.5 log unit steps after 2 consecutive TDMTS sessions were completed successfully, i.e., 0.32, 1.0, 3.2, 10.0 μg/kg/day. Each subject was scanned after the accumulated chronic dosage of AM2389 reached 0.32 mg/kg, i.e., 39–56 days after the chronic regimen began. Thereafter, to assess the extent of tolerance to the effects of the cannabinoid on TDMTS task performance, the effects of larger AM2389 doses (18.0–56.0 μg/kg) on TDMTS performance were determined in each subject. Larger doses of AM2389 were tested no more than once per week, with the 10.0 μg/kg chronic dose administered on intervening days.

Antagonist-precipitated withdrawal and abrupt discontinuation of AM2389

Two approaches were used to determine whether chronic AM2389 exposure produced physical dependence, as revealed by signs of withdrawal, after tolerance to its disruptive behavioral effects was evident. First, experiments were conducted to determine whether TDMTS task performance during the chronic AM2389 regimen was altered by administration of the CB1 antagonist rimonabant, presumably as a result of displacing AM2389 from CB1 receptors (i.e., antagonist-precipitated withdrawal). In these experiments, the effects of several doses of rimonabant (0.1–1.0 mg/kg) were determined by administering the antagonist 1-hr prior to the test session (i.e., 2-hr after daily treatment with 10.0 μg/kg AM2389). Rimonabant was studied in individual subjects no more than once per week. After tests with rimonabant concluded, chronic treatment with AM2389 continued for 10 days, after which vehicle solution replaced AM2389 as the daily treatment in all subjects. TDMTS performance was evaluated daily during the first 10 days following discontinuation, providing a second measure of withdrawal signs consequent to chronic AM2389 exposure. Finally, subjects were re-scanned 30 days after daily treatment with AM2389 was discontinued to evaluate the persistence of MRS- and MRI-related changes that may have occurred during the chronic regimen.

Magnetic resonance imaging and spectroscopy (MRI and MRS)

MRI data were acquired on a Siemens Magnetom 3T Trio (Tim syngo MR B17). A custom 5-channel phased-array head coil that was designed to optimize magnetic field homogeneity for rhesus macaque brains was used. On scan days, subjects were transported from the vivarium to an animal preparation room within the scanner suite. Subjects were sedated with 10 mg/kg ketamine i.m. for anesthesia induction, intubated, and then maintained on 1–1.2% isoflurane gas throughout the scanning procedures. A circulating warm-water blanket and fleece wrap were used to maintain body temperature. Monkeys were scanned in the prone position in an MRI-compatible primate holder. Vital signs including heart rate, respiration rate, and oxygen saturation (SPO2) were monitored throughout the procedure.

The image acquisition protocol included (1) a 3D MP-RAGE high resolution anatomic sequence (non-selective inversion-recovery time TI = 1,100 ms, TR = 2,530 ms, four echoes at TE = 3.69 ms; 7.64 ms; 9.72 ms; 11.8 ms, 128 sagittal slices, slice thickness = 1.3 mm, voxel size = 0.7 × 0.5 × 1.3 mm, FOV = 128 mm, flip angle = 70°, GRAPPA acceleration factor 2, acquisition time = 4 min 32 s. Anatomic images were then used to place a single 7 mm × 7 mm × 11 mm MRS voxel in the right mOFC – targeting an area encompassing 10mc, 10mr, 14c, 14r according to the Saleem and Logothetis macaque atlas (Saleem and Logothetis, 2012; see Figure 5A). Magnetic resonance spectra were next acquired using a J-resolved 2D-JPRESS sequence (Jensen et al., 2009; see also Kohut and Kaufman, 2021). Following manual shimming and optimization of the flip angles for water suppression and MRS acquisition, a TE-series comprised of 22 steps was acquired with initial TE = 35 ms, final TE = 350 ms, TE step = 15 ms (66.7 Hz J-resolved bandwidth). The mean TE of the TE-series = 192.5 ms matches the average TE of in vivo metabolite T2 values reported by Posse et al. (2007) for a field strength of 3T. Other acquisition details: TR = 2,190 ms, 8 averages per TE step, 2 kHz spectral bandwidth, 1,024 complex pairs per trace, 6:39 min total acquisition time. After acquisition of MRS data, a 10 mg/kg IV injection of Feraheme® (ferumoxytol) was administered and 5-min later, functional MRI data was acquired using a 2D Gradient-echo EPI sequence (TR = 1,750 ms, TE = 21 ms, 29 slices, slice thickness = 1.8 mm, interleaved, voxel size = 1.4 × 1.4 × 1.8 mm3, FOV = 130 mm, flip angle = 70°, GRAPPA acceleration factor 2, 344 volumes, acquisition time = 10 min 7 s).

FIGURE 2

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Session-wide mean (± SEM) titrated duration values (upper panel) and mean (± SEM) response rate values (lower panel) in the TDMTS task during control sessions prior to drug exposure (C; black circles), following vehicle administration (V), during acute dose-response determinations (black squares), during chronic dose-response determinations (gray squares) N = 4.

FIGURE 3

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Left panels: Effects of rimonabant administration on session-wide mean (± SEM) titrated duration values (upper-left panel) and mean (± SEM) response rate values (lower-left panel) in the TDMTS task before chronic exposure (BC; 1 mg/kg) and in dose-response determinations during chronic treatment (black diamonds; 0.1–1.0 mg/kg). For ease of comparison, values obtained during control sessions prior to drug exposure (C; black circles) are also shown. Right panels: Session-wide mean (± SEM) titrated duration values (upper-right panel) and mean (± SEM) response rate values (lower-right panel) in the TDMTS task during the last 5 sessions of chronic AM2389 exposure (DC; while circle) and during the 10 days following abrupt AM2389 discontinuation (black circles) N = 4.

FIGURE 4

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Group level (N = 4) fronto-cortical resting-state network spatial maps encompassing the mOFC, prefrontal cortex, and anterior cingulate (red-yellow). Green overlay shows regions identified using dual regression with greater connectivity during chronic AM2389 treatment relative to baseline (p < 0.05). There were no statistically significant differences between acute drug scan or discontinuation and baseline. See text for additional details.

FIGURE 5

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. (A) Representative voxel placement for spectroscopy measurement at medial orbitofrontal cortex (mOFC) in rhesus macaques. (B) Boxplots of N-acetylaspartate/creatine ratio (tNAA/tCrw) during baseline, acute administration of AM2389 (10 μg/kg), during chronic AM2389 treatment (total dose 0.32 mg/kg), and 30 days after treatment discontinuation. *p < 0.05 vs. Baseline (N = 4).

Data analysis Titrating-delay match-to-sample

Overall mean titrated duration values were calculated by averaging (± SEM) the titrated duration values across the 48-trial session. Response rate (responses/sec) during presentation of the sample stimulus was calculated by dividing the number of FR responses (20) by the time between the onset and offset of the sample stimulus; overall mean response rate for the session was calculated by averaging values (± SEM) across the number of completed trials. ED50 values were determined for both performance (average titrated delay) and response rate using log-linear interpolation in individual subject dose-response functions. Group ED50 values (with 95% confidence limits) were determined for comparisons of effects of acute and chronic AM2389 on performance and response rate. Log ED50 values were transformed to linear values for presentation.

Magnetic resonance spectroscopy

Spectra were processed as single spectra using TE-averaging (Hurd et al., 2004; Jensen et al., 2009). The TE-series were Fourier-transformed to frequency space using jMRUI v41, lightly apodized with a 2 Hz Lorentzian-Gaussian filter and summed. The resultant sum was fitted with LCModel using a dedicated simulated basis within the GAMMA/VESPA simulation software (Soher et al., 2007). The spectra were inspected visually in jMRUI and then fitted with the LCModel to quantify metabolites for spectra with water linewidths less than 10 Hz. The data were analyzed with a mixed-model repeated-measures ANOVA followed by Dunnet’s post hoc tests when appropriate. The tests were run for the ratios NAA/tCr and tNAA/tCr. tNAA was estimated by adding NAA and NAAG via a standard weighted-sum formula in error analysis (Bevington et al., 1993; Mintzopoulos et al., 2016). All statistical analyses were conducted using Stata (v14; StataCorp, College Station, Texas). Data from two subjects at the discontinuation timepoint were removed due to poor fits.

Functional magnetic resonance imaging

Image preprocessing included removing the first seven volumes, motion correction, spatial smoothing with a Gaussian Kernel of 2.5 mm FWHM, high pass temporal filtering using a 100 s filter, and slice timing correction conducted within the FSL software suite (FMRIB, Oxford University, United Kingdom). Processed data were then skull-stripped and registered to INIA macaque template space (Rohlfing et al., 2012) using the JIP software toolkit (Joe’s Image Program2, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard University, MA, United States). Group independent component analysis (ICA) was conducted to delineate resting state networks that overlapped the right mOFC voxel used in MRS and verified using spatial cross-correlation. Dual regression analysis was used to regress the group spatial maps into each subject’s 4D dataset to give a time course which was then regressed into the same 4D dataset for a subject-specific set of spatial maps (Smith et al., 2009). Permutation testing using the FSL randomize tool was used to compare differences across each condition. Family-wise error (FEW) correction with threshold-free cluster enhancement (TFCE) with a significance threshold of p < 0.05 was used to correct for multiple comparisons (Smith and Nichols, 2009).

Drugs

AM2389 was synthesized for these studies by the present authors (SJ, SPN, AM) in the Center for Drug Discovery at Northeastern University (Boston, MA, United States) using previously published procedures (Nikas et al., 2010). Rimonabant was provided by the NIDA Drug Supply Program (Rockville, MD, United States). AM2389 and rimonabant were prepared in a 20:20:60 mixture of 95% ethanol, Tween-80, and saline. All drug solutions were refrigerated and protected from light. Vehicle and drug doses were prepared in volumes of 0.5 ml/kg body weight or less and administered i.m. in calf or thigh muscle.

Results Titrating-delay match-to-sample

All subjects learned to touch the screen and progressed through matching-to-sample training to stable performance within approximately 60–75 training sessions. Group mean values for titrated delay (Figure 2, upper panel) and rates of responding during the FR20 sample response requirement (Figure 2, lower panel) are depicted by black circles. As shown, session-wide titrated delays averaged approximately 6 s and the FR20 requirement was completed, on average, at the rate of approximately 2.6 responses per sec. AM2389, given acutely, produced dose-dependent decreases in both measures and the highest acute dose, 10.0 μg/kg, abolished responding in all subjects (black squares). Following acute dose-response determinations, subjects were chronically exposed to AM2389 via a dosing regimen that, as described above, increased in a graded manner to the terminal daily dose of 10.0 μg/kg AM2389. Mean titrated delay values and FR20 response rates decreased initially but gradually recovered over the course of chronic treatment, presumably reflecting tolerance to the behaviorally disruptive effects of AM2389. Dose-response functions were redetermined when the total AM2389 dosage administered during the chronic regimen reached 0.32 mg/kg which, depending on the subject’s progression through dose escalation, took 39–56 days of once-daily injection. As depicted by the gray squares in the left panels of Figure 2, mean titrated delay values at the time of dose-response re-determination (DC) approximated pre-chronic control values whereas the mean rate of FR20 responding was approximately 70% of pre-chronic response rates. Administration of higher AM2389 doses (18.0–56.0 μg/kg) revealed a > 30-fold rightward shift in the position of the dose-response function [ED50 (CLs); acute = 1.68 μg/kg (0.71, 3.95), chronic = 36.55 μg/kg (25.43, 52.54)], indicating considerable tolerance to the effects of AM2389 on TDMTS performance. The largest dose of AM2389 tested during chronic conditions (56.0 μg/kg) abolished task performance in 2 of 4 subjects.

The left panels of Figure 3 present the effects of rimonabant on mean titrated delay values (upper-left panel) and response rate (lower-left panel) before (1 mg/kg; white diamonds) and during (0.1–1 mg/kg; black diamonds) chronic AM2389 exposure. Rimonabant failed to disrupt mean titrated values either before or during chronic AM2389 exposure (cf. Figure 2, black circle) but did alter rates of responding. Thus, the highest dose of rimonabant, 1.0 mg/kg decreased the mean response rate to approximately 50% of values when studied before the chronic regimen. Lower doses of rimonabant (0.1 and 0.32 mg/kg) also were evaluated during the chronic AM2389 regimen and had differing effects on control values for response rate during the chronic regimen (cf., Figure 2, gray square). Thus, whereas the intermediate dose of rimonabant (0.32 mg/kg) did not appreciably alter those values, the lowest dose of rimonabant (0.1 mg/kg), increased responding sufficiently to recover prechronic response rates, albeit with greater individual variability than observed under other conditions.

The right panels of Figure 3 show the effects of abrupt AM2389 discontinuation on titrated delay (upper-right panel) and response rate (lower-right panel). The white circles and shaded area represent, respectively, the average across the last 5 sessions of the preceding chronic AM2389 condition. As shown by the black circles, mean titrated delay values were reduced during the session on the day following discontinuation, i.e., 27 h after the last injection of AM2389. The disruption in performance increased during the second day following discontinuation, as evident by an approximately ∼60% decrease in titrated delay values. Thereafter, mean titrated delay values gradually recovered across sessions and, by the 10th day following AM2389 discontinuation, began to stabilize at levels of control performance that previously were observed prior to and during chronic exposure (cf. black circle in upper panel of Figure 2). Rates of responding also were disrupted by discontinuation of treatment with AM2389; however, peak disruption was observed 4 days after AM2389 discontinuation. Like titrated delay values, recovery of responding followed and mean response rates approximated previously observed values by the end of the 10-session condition. However, unlike titrated delay values, response rate did not approximate pre-chronic levels (cf. black circle in lower panel of Figure 2) by the end of the 10-session condition.

Functional magnetic resonance imaging

Independent component analysis identified a prefrontal IC encompassing the mOFC region of interest (see Figure 4; warm colors). This network contained bilateral mOFC, prefrontal cortical regions, and anterior cingulate cortex and overlapped with the mOFC voxel placed for MRS analysis. The dual regression analysis suggested that connectivity decreased from the pre-AM2389 baseline following acute treatment with AM2389 (10 μg/kg; p = 0.08). However, chronic exposure to AM2389 (0.32 mg/kg) did not exacerbate this effect but, instead, further increased connectivity within the network (green, p < 0.05), especially in mOFC, anterior cingulate, prefrontal cortex, and caudate. However, these effects of chronic AM2389 on functional connectivity were not enduring and were no longer statistically significant 30 days after discontinuation of the chronic regimen (p > 0.05).

Magnetic resonance spectroscopy

Figure 5 shows the ratio of tNAA/tCrw in the mOFC voxel under different experimental conditions (pre-drug baseline, following acute treatment with 10 μg/kg AM2389, after exposure to a total dosage of 0.32 mg/kg AM2389 during chronic treatment, and 30 days following discontinuation of the chronic regimen). ANOVA identified a significant effect of treatment [F(3,7) = 13.35; p = 0.003] with post hoc tests showing that the tNAA/tCrw ratio following acute administration (10 μg/kg) and during the chronic treatment condition (0.32 mg/kg) were significantly decreased relative to baseline ratios (both p’s < 0.02). However, this effect was transient as tNAA/tCrw ratio in mOFC 30 days after discontinuation of AM2389 treatment did not differ from baseline levels (p > 0.05).

Discussion

In the present study, acute administration of the CB1 full agonist AM2389 produced dose-related disruptions in behavioral and neurochemical measures (tNAA/tCrw ratio) but, notwithstanding a trend toward decreased local frontal connectivity, did not significantly alter connectivity in the mOFC network. During chronic exposure, tolerance developed to the effects of AM2389 on both motoric performance (response rates under the FR20 schedule of food reinforcement) and memorial function (titrated delay in the matching-to-sample procedure; TDMTS) but not to its neurochemical effects in the mOFC. Finally, whereas the administration of the CB1 antagonist rimonabant during chronic treatment disrupted motoric performance but not memorial function, discontinuation of the chronic AM2389 regimen disrupted both types of behavior. Overall, the present findings are consistent with the results of previous studies in humans and laboratory animals showing that, first, cannabinoid agonists may produce adverse effects on short-term memory processes (e.g., Abush and Akirav, 2012; Crane et al., 2013; Verrico et al., 2014; Stringfield and Torregrossa, 2021) and second, tolerance develops to such behavioral effects of cannabinoid agonists over the course of chronic exposure (Ramaekers et al., 2011). The magnitude of tolerance observed here was substantial (1.5 log unit rightward shift) and is comparable in magnitude to that reported in previous behavioral studies of cannabinoid tolerance and cross-tolerance in non-human primates (e.g., McMahon, 2011; Desai et al., 2013). It is interesting that tolerance to the effects of the daily dosage of AM2389 appeared more complete on memorial function than on motoric performance. Thus, whereas TDMTS recovered fully during the chronic AM2389 regimen, response rates recovered to only 70% of pre-chronic values and, during dose-response re-determinations, were markedly disrupted by doses of AM2389 that had only moderate effects on titrated delay values. Previous studies have shown that the extent to which acutely administered cannabinoids disrupt performance on cognitive tasks may vary with the difficulty of the task, i.e., cognitive load (Kangas et al., 2016). The present results indicate that tolerance to the disruptive effects of chronically administered cannabinoid agonists also may vary with the class of behavior being measured, i.e., cognitive vs. motoric task performance.

Although the precise defining features and severity of cannabis withdrawal continue to be a subject of inquiry (e.g., Budney and Hughes, 2006; Schlienz et al., 2017), it is now commonly accepted that withdrawal symptoms can emerge following the abrupt discontinuation of cannabis products, especially in high-intake chronic users. This view was codified in the DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) with the inclusion of Cannabis Use Disorder which, for the first time, included diagnostic criteria for cannabis dependence. Moreover, it is widely recognized that such withdrawal discomfort can include pronounced disruptions in cognitive function (Cousijn and van Duijvenvoorde, 2018). The present results showing that discontinuation of AM2389 treatment in the present studies was followed by decrements in short-term memory is consistent with this understanding of cannabis withdrawal. Such effects were most severe on the second day after treatment was discontinued, which is in line with the long duration of action reported for AM2389 (Järbe et al., 2012). The recovery of task performance over the course of approximately 5–10 days also comports well with findings from a previous study of human memory processes in adolescent and young adults showing that 1 month (but as brief as 1 week) of abstinence from cannabis use was associated with improvements in memory task performance (Schuster et al., 2018; see also Scott et al., 2018).

The failure of the CB1 receptor antagonist rimonabant to produce withdrawal effects comparable to or, depending on dose, more intense than those observed following discontinuation of the AM2389 regimen was surprising. A defining feature of drug dependence resulting from chronic exposure to other classes of drugs, for example, opioids or benzodiazepines (e.g., Gellert and Holtzman, 1979; Lukas and Griffiths, 1982) is the ability of receptor antagonists to precipitate overt, pronounced signs of withdrawal, and previous studies have shown that the stimulus properties of 24 h abstinence after chronic treatment with Δ9-THC or AM2389 overlap with those of rimonabant in non-human primates (Stewart and McMahon, 2010; Kangas et al., 2020). Studies in rodents and non-human primates that are chronically treated with Δ9-THC or other CB1 agonists have further suggested that rimonabant-precipitated withdrawal can be characterized by overt signs including paw tremor and head shakes (Verberne et al., 1981; Cook et al., 1998; Stewart and McMahon, 2010). However, this remains controversial, as such behavioral disturbances can be produced by rimonabant alone (including psychiatric side effects that led to its withdrawal from market; Nguyen et al., 2019) and are not always apparent in subjects treated with cannabinoids like Δ9-THC or AM2389 (Kangas et al., 2020). Interestingly, studies in cannabinoid-treated rodents also have revealed time- and dose-dependent changes in motor activity consequent to rimonabant injection that could serve as a more reliable indicator of cannabinoid withdrawal (Huang et al., 2009; Missig et al., 2022; Paronis et al., 2022). With the latter findings in mind, it is noteworthy that, in the present study, lower doses of rimonabant produced considerable increases in fixed-ratio responding in AM2389-treated monkeys, likely reflecting a mild agitation or motor stimulation. Possibly, assays that are particularly sensitive to such changes in motor activity or assessment of daily patterns of locomotor activity during cannabinoid treatment and discontinuation will provide more consistent demonstrations of antagonist-precipitated cannabinoid withdrawal than are currently available.

Neuroimaging studies in human subjects suggest that use of cannabis during adolescence may lead to abnormalities in brain function, structure, and chemistry in several regions related to cognitive processing (reviewed by Lichenstein et al., 2022; Ma et al., 2022). Studies conducted in adolescent cannabis users have often focused on mOFC, a brain region that undergoes extensive pruning and reorganization during adolescence (Sturman and Moghaddam, 2011) and that is thought to play a role in maintaining drug use in addiction (London et al., 2000; Winstanley, 2007; Schoenbaum and Shaham, 2008). The mOFC also has been linked to performance on cognitive tasks including delayed match-to-sample performance (Lamar et al., 2004). Accordingly, ICA–which revealed a fronto-cortical functional network that primarily encompasses the mOFC, prefrontal cortex, and anterior cingulate—and an MRS voxel in the mOFC were utilized in the present study to probe changes in brain function and chemistry during and after chronic AM2389 treatment. The present results, illustrating greater functional connectivity within a fronto-cortical network during chronic AM2389 treatment, are consistent with human studies showing a relationship between the extent of cannabis use and frontal connectivity (Houck et al., 2013; Orr et al., 2013; Lopez-Larson et al., 2015). For example, increased OFC functional connectivity with other frontal and motor regions (Lopez-Larson et al., 2015) as well as with dorsal striatal regions (Zimmermann et al., 2018) has been demonstrated previously in heavy cannabis users when compared to healthy controls. Importantly, greater OFC connectivity with frontal and subcortical regions also has been associated with greater resistance to behavioral change in high-risk adolescent cannabis users (Feldstein Ewing et al., 2017), consistent with the view that alterations in this network may be a key component of compromised executive control in adolescent cannabis users. In the present study, changes in RSFC within this network could be related to changes in performance on the TDMTS task. For example, RSFC within the fronto-cortical network showed a non-significant trend for decreased connectivity following acute treatment with a behaviorally disruptive dose of AM2389 (10 ug/kg). In contrast, RSFC increased within the frontal network following a regimen of daily AM2389 administration that induced tolerance to its performance–disrupting effects. It is tempting to speculate that increased connectivity among frontal regions during chronic AM2389 treatment represents a recruitment of brain regions that helped to overcome the disruptive behavioral effects of AM2389 that were evident after acute administration. These effects were not persistent, however, as functional connectivity following treatment discontinuation was able to recover to pre-treatment levels.

The combined use of fMRI and MRS in the same subjects provided the opportunity to gain insights into the neurochemical mechanisms that may contribute to drug-induced changes in functional connectivity (Kohut and Kaufman, 2021). Here, we showed that the tNAA/tCrw ratio in mOFC decreased in a dose-dependent manner during AM2389 treatment but returned to baseline levels following discontinuation. We focused on NAA levels because they are thought to be a marker of neuronal health and integrity and have been shown to be altered in substance use disorders (Magalhaes, 2005), including cannabis use disorder. For example, increased striatal NAA has been positively correlated to subjective reports of “high” following acute THC administration to occasional cannabis users (Mason et al., 2021). On the other hand, decreased NAA concentration has been reported in frontal regions such as anterior cingulate cortex (Prescot et al., 2011) and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Hermann et al., 2007) in chronic cannabis users suggesting that the effects of cannabis on brain NAA may be region-dependent or change with chronic exposure. Further, Sung et al. (2013) showed decreased frontal NAA levels in adolescents that use methamphetamine + marijuana compared to levels in methamphetamine only users or healthy controls; NAA ratios were also inversely related to lifetime and duration of marijuana use. The relationship between neurochemical environment in a given brain region and its functional connectivity is likely to be complex and influenced by multiple factors, Lord et al. (2017) described a quadratic relationship between NAA concentration and functional connectivity in posterior cingulate cortex of healthy volunteers in which functional connectivity and NAA decreased linearly before increasing again. While a causative influence of NAA on functional connectivity (or vice versa) cannot be proven from data in the present study, it is noteworthy that the effects of AM2389 administration generally followed this same pattern. Further studies that target other metabolites in frontal regions may shed greater light on how cannabinoids alter the relationship between neurochemical indices and functional connectivity.

There are several caveats and limitations of these studies that deserve mention. First, all adolescent rhesus macaques examined were female. As greater CB1 density in several brain regions has been consistently reported in males than females in laboratory animal studies (e.g., Rubino et al., 2008; Reich et al., 2009; Burston et al., 2010; Mateos et al., 2011), it will be important for future studies to determine whether the present findings can be replicated in adolescent male monkeys. Second, relating hormonal phases to behavioral and neuroimaging endpoints was beyond the scope of the present studies and, therefore, not systematically assessed. Interestingly, although these subjects had not begun menses prior to the initiation of these studies, first menses and subsequent cycling were observed in all subjects at various points during these studies, confirming that these experiments indeed were occurring during the intended developmental stage. Also, magnetic resonance spectroscopy conducted at 3T field strength was not sensitive enough in non-human primates to distinguish resonance peaks for other neurochemicals of interest such as glutamate, y-aminobutyric acid (GABA), and glutamine. Future studies using other acquisition techniques or ultra-high field scanners to enhance the signal of such metabolites will help further characterize the neurochemical environment in the mOFC and related brain regions during chronic drug administration (Ma et al., 2022). Next, although accuracy in the TDMTS task was adversely affected by acute AM2389 administration as well as discontinuation of chronic treatment, it was noteworthy that all rewards earned by subjects were indeed consumed. Despite this, a decrease in motivation for the milk reinforcer cannot be ruled out as the behavioral mechanism of observed task disruption and, as such, the effects of chronic AM2389 treatment and discontinuation upon TDMTS performance maintained by a non-food consequence will be necessary to examine this possibility. Finally, it is important to note that inasmuch as these studies employed a within-subject behavioral and neuroimaging design during pre-drug exposure, an untreated control group was not included during or following chronic exposure. While a drug-free control group would have allowed for interesting comparisons of behavioral and neuroimaging endpoints during these phases of this research, the longitudinal design of the present study nonetheless clearly reveals modulation of both cognitive and MRS/fMRI metrics during acute and chronic cannabinoid exposure and, as well, their normalization following its discontinuation.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The animal study was reviewed and approved by McLean Hospital Animal Care and Use Committee.

Author contributions

SK, JB, BF, JJ, and BK participated in research design. SK, CZ, JJ, and BK conducted the experiments. SJ, SN, and AM contributed the new reagents or analytic tools. DM, LC, BF, SK, and BK performed the data analysis. SK, LC, DM, JB, and BK contributed the writing of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by NIH/NIDA grants K01-DA039306 (SK), K01-DA035974 (BK), and R01-DA047575 (JB/SK/BK).

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Bonnie Adams and Charlene Comeau for their assistance with the collection of imaging data and Jess Nardolillo and David S. Jacobs with collection of the behavioral data. We would also like to thank Kenroy Cayetano who designed and built the head coil used in this study.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes ^ http://www.jmrui.eu/ ^ http://www.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/~jbm/jip/ References

Abush, H., and Akirav, I. (2012). Short- and long-term cognitive effects of chronic cannabinoids administration in late-adolescence rats. PLoS One 7:e31731. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0031731

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Desk reference to the diagnostic criteria from DSM-5. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Google Scholar

Bava, S., Jacobus, J., Mahmood, O., Yang, T. T., and Tapert, S. F. (2010). Neurocognitive correlates of white matter quality in adolescent substance users. Brain Cogn. 72, 347–354. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.10.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bevington, P. R., Robinson, D. K., Blair, J. M., Mallinckrodt, A. J., and McKay, S. (1993). Data Reduction and Error Analysis for the Physical Sciences. Comput. Phys. 7:415.

Google Scholar

Budney, A. J., and Hughes, J. R. (2006). The cannabis withdrawal syndrome. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 19, 233–238. doi: 10.1097/01.yco.0000218592.00689.e5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burston, J. J., Wiley, J. L., Craig, A. A., Selley, D. E., and Sim-Selley, L. J. (2010). Regional enhancement of cannabinoid CB1 receptor desensitization in female adolescent rats following repeated Delta-tetrahydrocannabinol exposure. Br. J. Pharmacol. 161, 103–112. doi: 10.1111/j.1476-5381.2010.00870.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cadoni, C., Pisanu, A., Solinas, M., Acquas, E., and Di Chiara, G. (2001). Behavioural sensitization after repeated exposure to Delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol and cross-sensitization with morphine. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 158, 259–66. doi: 10.1007/s002130100875

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Camchong, J., Lim, K. O., and Kumra, S. (2017). Adverse effects of cannabis on adolescent brain development: A longitudinal study. Cereb. Cortex 27, 1922–1930.

Google Scholar

Cha, Y. M., Jones, K. H., Kuhn, C. M., Wilson, W. A., and Swartzwelder, H. S. (2007). Sex differences in the effects of delta9-tetrahydrocannabinol on spatial learning in adolescent and adult rats. Behav. Pharmacol. 18, 563–569.

Google Scholar

Cheetham, A., Allen, N. B., Whittle, S., Simmons, J. G., Yucel, M., and Lubman, D. I. (2012). Orbitofrontal volumes in early adolescence predict initiation of cannabis use: A 4-year longitudinal and prospective study. Biol. Psychiatry 71, 684–692. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.10.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, H., Skosnik, P. D., Pruce, B. J., Brumbaugh, M. S., Vollmer, J. M., Fridberg, D. J., et al. (2014). Resting state functional magnetic resonance imaging reveals distinct brain activity in heavy cannabis users: A multi-voxel pattern analysis. J. Psychopharmacol. 28, 1030–1040. doi: 10.1177/0269881114550354

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Churchwell, J. C., Lopez-Larson, M., and Yurgelun-Todd, D. A. (2010). Altered Frontal Cortical Volume and Decision Making in Adolescent Cannabis Users. Front. Psychol. 1:225. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cook, S. A., Lowe, J. A., and Martin, B. R. (1998). CB1 receptor antagonist precipitates withdrawal in mice exposed to Delta9-tetrahydrocannabinol. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 285, 1150–1156.

Google Scholar

Cousijn, J., and van Duijvenvoorde, A. C. K. (2018). Cognitive and Mental Health Predictors of Withdrawal Severity During an Active Attempt to Cut Down Cannabis Use. Front. Psychiatry 11:301. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

留言 (0)

沒有登入
gif