‘Pragmatics First’: Animal Communication and the Evolution of Language

Arnold, K., and K. Zuberbühler. 2006. Semantic combinations in primate calls. Nature 441: 303.

Article  Google Scholar 

Arnold, K., and K. Zuberbühler. 2008. Meaningful call combinations in a non-human primate. Current Biology 18: R202–R204.

Article  Google Scholar 

Arnold, K., and K. Zuberbühler. 2012. Call combinations in monkeys: compositional or idiomatic expressions? Brain and Language 120 (3): 303–309.

Article  Google Scholar 

Arnold, K., and K. Zuberbühler. 2013. Female putty-nosed monkeys use experimentally altered contextual information to disambiguate the cause of male alarm calls. PLoS ONE 8 (6): e65660.

Article  Google Scholar 

Arnold, K., and D. Bar-On. 2020. Primate pragmatics, expressive behavior, and the evolution of language. Animal Behavior and Cognition 7 (2): 117–130.

Article  Google Scholar 

Asif, N. 2022. Minimal theory of mind – a millikanian approach. Synthese 200 (2): 1–26.

Article  Google Scholar 

Bach, K. 1997. The semantics-pragmatics distinction. Linguistische Berichte 8: 33–50.

Google Scholar 

Bach, K. 2004. Minding the gap. In The Semantics/Pragmatics distinction, ed. Claudia Bianchi. 27–43. CSLI Publications.

Bar-On, D. 2013. Origins of meaning: must we ‘go Gricean’? Mind & Language 28 (3): 342–375.

Article  Google Scholar 

Bar-On, D. 2021. How to do things with nonwords: pragmatics, biosemantics, and origins of language in animal communication. Biology & Philosophy 36 (6): 1–25.

Article  Google Scholar 

Bar-On, D., and R. Moore. 2017. Pragmatic interpretation and signaler-receiver asymmetries in animal communication. In The Routledge handbook of philosophy of animal minds, ed. K. Andrews and J. Beck, 291–300. Routledge.

Chapter  Google Scholar 

Bermudez, J. 2003. Thinking without words. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Book  Google Scholar 

Berwick, R., and N. Chomsky. 2017. Why only us? Cambridge MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar 

Breheny, R. 2006. Communication and folk psychology. Mind & Language 21: 74–107.

Article  Google Scholar 

Breheny, R. 2019. Scalar implicatures. In The oxford handbook of experimental semantics and pragmatics, ed. Napoleon. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Burling, R. 2005. The talking ape. New York: Oxford University Press.

Book  Google Scholar 

Carnap, R. 1942. Introduction to semantics. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar 

Carston, R. 1998. Informativeness, relevance, and scalar implicatures. In Relevance theory, ed. R. Carston and S. Uchida. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

Chapter  Google Scholar 

Carston, R. 2008. Linguistic communication and the semantics/pragmatics distinction. Synthese 165: 321–345.

Article  Google Scholar 

Carston, R. 2015. Pragmatics and semantics. In The Oxford Handbook of Pragmatics, 453–472. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar 

Corballis, M. 2002. From Hand to Mouth: the origins of Language. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

Book  Google Scholar 

Crockford, C., et al. 2015. An intentional vocalization draws others’ attention. Animal Cognition 18 (3): 581–501.

Article  Google Scholar 

Eckardt, W., and K. Zuberbühler. 2004. Cooperation and competition in two forest monkeys. Behavioral Ecology 15: 400–411.

Article  Google Scholar 

Eiteljoerge, S., et al. 2018. Some pieces are missing: implicature production in children. Frontiers in Psychology 9: 1–16. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01928.

Article  Google Scholar 

Fitch, T. 2010. The evolution of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Book  Google Scholar 

Grice, P. 1957. Meaning. Philosophical Review 66 (3): 377–388.

Article  Google Scholar 

Grice, P. 1968. Utterer’s meaning, sentence-meaning, and word-meaning. Foundations of Language 4: 225–242.

Google Scholar 

Heintz, C., and T. Scott-Phillips. 2022. Expression Unleashed Behavioral and Brain Sciences , vol 46 

Hobaiter, K., and R. Byrne. 2014. The meanings of chimpanzee gestures. Current Biology 24 (14): 1596–1600.

Article  Google Scholar 

Hopkins, et al. 2007. Chimpanzees differentially produce novel voclizations to capture the attention of a human. Animal Behaviour 73: 281–286.

Article  Google Scholar 

Horn, L. 1989. A natural history of negation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Google Scholar 

Hurford, J. 2007. The origins of meaning: Language in the light of evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar 

Katsos, N., and D. Bishop. 2011. Pragmatic tolerance. Cognition 120 (1): 67–81.

Article  Google Scholar 

Liebal, K., et al. 2004. To move or not to move: how apes adjust to the attentional state of others. Interaction Studies 5 (2): 199–219.

Article  Google Scholar 

Maynard-Smith, J., and D. Harper. 2003. Animal signals. New York: Oxford University Press.

Book  Google Scholar 

McNeill, D. 2012. How Language Began: gesture and Speech in Human Evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Book  Google Scholar 

Millikan, R. 1984. Language, Thought, and other Biological categories. Cambridge, Mass: The MIT Press.

Book  Google Scholar 

Millikan, R. 1995. Pushmi-Pullyu representations. In Philosophical perspectives, ed. J. Tomberlin. vol. 9 185–200. Atascedero CA: Ridgeview Publishing.

Google Scholar 

Millikan, R. 2004. Varieties of meaning. Cambridge, Mass: The MIT Press.

Book  Google Scholar 

Millikan, R. 2017. Beyond concepts. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Book  Google Scholar 

Moore, R. 2017. Convergent minds: ostension, inference and Grice’s third clause. Interface Focus 7 (3): 20160107.

Article  Google Scholar 

Moore, R. 2018. Gricean communication, language development, and animal minds. Philosophy Compass 13 (12).

Oller, D., and U. Griebel. 2008. Evolution of communicative flexibility. Cambridge: MIT University Press.

Book  Google Scholar 

Origgi, G., and D. Sperber. 2000. Evolution, communication, and the proper function of language. In Evolution and the human mind, ed. P. Carruthers and A. Chamberlain, 140–169. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Chapter  Google Scholar 

Origgi, G., and D. Sperber. 2010. A pragmatic perspective on the evolution of language. In The Evolution of Human Language, ed. R. Larson, et al. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Recanati, F. 2002. Does linguistic communication rest on inference? Mind and Language 17: 105–126.

Article  Google Scholar 

Recanati, F. 2004. Literal meaning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar 

Recanati, F. 2010. Truth-Conditional Pragmatics. Oxford Clarendon Press.

Schel, A.M., et al. 2013. Chimpanzee alarm call production meets key criteria for intentionality. PLoS ONE 8 (10): 1–11.

Article  Google Scholar 

Schlenker, P. 2016. The semantics-pragmatics interface. In Formal semantics, ed. M. Aloni and P. Dekker, 664–727. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Chapter  Google Scholar 

Schlenker, P., et al. 2014. Monkey semantics: two “dialects” of Campbell’s monkey alarm calls. Linguistics and Philosophy 37 (6): 439–501.

Article  Google Scholar 

Schlenker, P., et al. 2015. Pyow-hack revisited: two analyses of putty-nosed monkey alarm calls. Lingua 171: 1–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2015.10.002.

Article  Google Scholar 

Schlenker, P., et al. 2016a. What do monkey calls mean? Trends in Cognitive Science 20 (12): 894–904.

Article  Google Scholar 

Schlenker, P., et al. 2016b. Formal monkey linguistics. Theoretical Linguistics 42 (1–2): 1–90.

Article  Google Scholar 

Scott-Phillips, T.C. 2015. Speaking our minds. London: Palgrave Macmillian.

Book  Google Scholar 

Seyfarth, R.M., and D. Cheney. 2003. Meaning and emotion in animal vocalizations. Annals of the NY Academy of Science 1000: 32–55.

Article  Google Scholar 

Seyfarth, R., and D. Cheney. 2017. The origin of meaning in animal signals. Animal Behavior 124: 339–346.

Article  Google Scholar 

Sperber, D. 2000. Metarepresentation in an evolutionary perspective. In Metarepresentations: a multidisciplinary perspective, ed. D. Sperber, 117–137. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Chapter  Google Scholar 

Sperber, D., and D. Wilson. 2015. Beyond speaker meaning. Croatian Journal of Philosophy XV: 117–149.

Google Scholar 

Sperber, D., and D. Wilson. 1986/95. Relevance: Communication and Cognition. (2nd edition 1995.) Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

Tomasello, M. 2008. Origins of human communication. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.

Book  Google Scholar 

Towsend, S., et al. 2017. Exorcising Grice’s ghost: an empirical approach to studying intentional communication in animals. Biological Review 92: 1427–1433.

Article  Google Scholar 

Vonk, J. (ed.). 2020. Animal cognition and behavior 7(2) special issue: Communication in nonhumans: the forty year anniversary of Seyfarth, Cheney and Marler.

Warren, E., and J. Call. 2022. Inferential communication: bridging the gap between intentional and ostensive communication in non-human primates. Frontiers in Psychology 14: 1–17.

Google Scholar 

Wheeler, B.C., and J. Fischer. 2012. Functionally referential signs. Evolutionary Anthropology 21 (5): 195–205. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.21319.

Article  Google Scholar 

Wilson, D. 2005. New directions for research on pragmatics and modularity. Lingua 115: 1129–1146.

Article  Google Scholar 

留言 (0)

沒有登入
gif