Cognitive reserve over the life course and risk of dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis

1 Introduction

The public health challenge of dementia has profound physical, psychological, social, and economic implications for individuals afflicted with the disease and their caregivers, families, and broader society (Aranda et al., 2021; Alzheimer’s Association, 2022). This disease stands as the leading cause of disability among older adults globally. As of 2015, approximately 50 million people globally were affected by dementia, constituting about 5% of the elderly population. Projects indicate that by 2030, the number of individuals with dementia will rise to 82 million, and by 2050, to 152 million (WHO, 2019). Given the current absence of effective treatment for dementia, it is crucial to identify pertinent risk factors and implement appropriate preventative measures (WHO, 2019).

According to research on dementia prevention (WHO, 2019; Livingston et al., 2020), cognitive reserve (CR) offers comprehensive protection against the onset of this disease. The concept of CR was initially proposed by Stern (2002), referring to the adaptability of cognitive processes. This adaptability explains why certain cognitive capacities or daily functioning are more or less susceptible to brain aging, pathology, or injury (Stern et al., 2020). This phenomenon may elucidate why some patients display dementia-related brain damage without accompanying clinical symptoms (Livingston et al., 2020). CR is a dynamic construct influenced by both intrinsic factors, such as intelligence quotient (IQ), and lifelong exposure (Stern et al., 2020). Directly assessment of CR is challenging because it is shaped by the accumulation of several protective and risk factors. The use of proxies for CR is still controversial. Researchers usually use proxy indicators such as education, occupation, cognitive activity, and social engagement to assess CR (Stern, 2009). However, as the protective role of lifestyle factors against dementia risk is gradually being explored, factors such as physical activity (Reas et al., 2019) have also been used to assess the relationship with cognitive function and are discussed together with CR-related indicators (Najar et al., 2019). Although physical activity may also be one of the favorable factors to reduce the risk of dementia according to existing studies (Palta et al., 2019; Hwang et al., 2023), we preferred to use the original concept of CR proposed by Stern (2009) in this study considering the rigor of the study. Referring to the Reserve and Resilience meetings sponsored by the NIA, we identified the CR proxies in this study as occupational complexity, educational attainment, leisure activities, cognitive activities, and social connections, and other measures (e.g., socioeconomic status) that could reflect the above proxies were also taken into account if they appeared.

Recent studies have identified certain CR traits associated with a lower risk of dementia and slower rates of memory decline during natural aging (Wang et al., 2019; Ding et al., 2020; Foverskov et al., 2020; Duffner et al., 2022). However, other studies have reported no statistically significant difference in dementia risk associated with these CR proxies (Prince et al., 2012; Sörman et al., 2014; Dekhtyar et al., 2016; Rusmaully et al., 2017; Takasugi et al., 2019; Zhang et al., 2021). Additionally, preventing dementia is a long-term process, and the impact of the effect of CR on dementia risk may vary across different life stages (Xu et al., 2019). While a meta-analysis (Hyun et al., 2022) established a link between education and occupational complexity in early and middle life and the risk of dementia, there still exists a dearth of literature on the relationship between various CR proxies and dementia risk at each stage of the lifespan. The point in the life course and the specific CR proxies that contribute to dementia risk reduction remains unclear. This review plans to investigate the relationship between CR levels and the risk of dementia at various life stages and provide a foundation for early dementia detection and prevention.

2 Methods

This systematic review and meta-analysis was registered in the International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews (PROSPERO) with the registration number CRD42022330713. The review procedure was governed by the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) checklist (Page et al., 2021).

2.1 Search strategy

A computerized systematic literature search was performed in PubMed, Embase, Web of Science, and MEDLINE databases, from their inception to June 3, 2023. The search terms included a combination of keywords related to CR, dementia, and risk. For CR, the following keywords were used: cognitive reserve OR brain reserve OR cognitive capacity OR neural reserve OR brain maintenance OR cognitive resilience OR brain resilience OR education OR occupation OR leisure activity OR cognitive activity OR social connection. The dementia-related keywords included dementia OR dement OR Alzheimer. Lastly, the following keywords were applied for risk: risk OR hazard ratio (HR). The search was limited to human studies and full-text original articles, with no language restrictions. The full search strategy is provided in Supplementary Appendix A.

2.2 Eligibility criteria

Studies were required to meet the following inclusion criteria: (a) Evaluation of CR in early, middle, and later life using CR proxy indicators (Stern, 2009), which encompassed measures of socioeconomic status (e.g., occupational complexity, educational attainment, leisure activity, cognitive activity, and social connection). For this study, early life was defined as birth to 30 years old, middle life as 30–60 years old, and late-life as over 60 years old; (b) Analysis of the relationship between life course CR accumulation and the risk of dementia; and (c) Utilization of a longitudinal study design. Studies were excluded if: (a) Participants exhibited dementia or other neurological diseases at baseline; (b) Participants had mental disorders at baseline; (c) Non-human subjects were included; and (d) Papers contained incomplete information, systematic reviews, meta-analyses, or reports from meetings or congresses.

2.3 Study selection

The searched articles were input into Rayyan (Ouzzani et al., 2016), a web-based tool designed for the efficient management of selected articles. Following the predefined inclusion and exclusion criteria, two reviewers independently conducted an initial screening of the titles and abstracts. Articles that passed this initial screening underwent a secondary screening upon acquisition of the full text to determine their eligibility for inclusion.

2.4 Quality assessment

The Newcastle-Ottawa scale (NOS) (Stang, 2010), a well-established method for assessing the quality of cohort studies, was utilized to evaluate the quality of these studies from three perspectives: selection, comparability, and outcome. Each part has a corresponding score, and the standards set by the Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (AHRQ) (Wells et al., 2021) were applied to the NOS score. According to the scores for each section, the articles’ quality ranged from good to fair to poor.

2.5 Data extraction

Data extraction was performed independently by two investigators, with any discrepancies resolved through discussion with a third researcher to reach a consensus. Information extracted from the selected studies included publication details (author, year, and country), participant characteristics (age, gender, and sample size), cohort details (source and follow-up period), exposure information (measurement of CR), outcome data (dementia diagnosis), and adjusted factors. Summary statistics, such as HR and their corresponding 95% confidence intervals (CIs), were recorded. In instances of missing data, attempts were made to contact the original authors for retrieval.

2.6 Data synthesis

Considering that some commands exceeded the capacity of Stata SE 15 while others did not meet the requirements of Stata MP 17, the statistical analysis of the findings from the included studies was conducted using both Stata SE 15 and Stata MP 17 (Stata Corp LP, USA). A narrative synthesis of the findings from the included studies was performed. Additionally, a meta-analysis was conducted using the inverse variance weighted method, pooling the extracted Hazard ratios (HR) and their corresponding 95% confidence intervals using a random effects mode (Borenstein et al., 2010). Statistical heterogeneity was quantified using the I2 statistic (Jonathan et al., 2022). Publication bias was assessed using funnel plots and Begg and Egger tests (Egger et al., 1997).

2.7 Sensitivity analyses

To assess the stability and reliability of the results, sensitivity analyses were conducted as follows: (a) The stability of the overall results was verified by analyzing the same data using both fixed-effect and random-effect models; (b) The impact of individual studies on the overall results was assessed by calculating pooled risk estimates and heterogeneity after sequentially removing each study from the analyses; (c) To address the potential influence of CR Proxies on the results, the studies were stratified based on different categories in early-life, middle-life, and late-life; (d) To evaluate the influence of publication bias and subjective judgment in the inclusion and exclusion of literature on the results, the trim-and-fill method was employed; and (e) To consider the potential effect of study quality on the results, the analysis was restricted to studies with a low risk of bias.

3 Results 3.1 Search results

Figure 1 illustrates the various phases of the systematic search and article selection during the literature review process. A total of 33,241 papers were identified for this review. Following deduplication, 15,627 potentially relevant citations were identified for screening of titles and abstracts. Subsequently, 283 full-text papers were selected for in-depth analysis. Based on the inclusion and exclusion criteria, 256 papers were excluded. The reasons for exclusion are detailed in the PRISMA flowchart (Figure 1) and Supplementary Appendix Table A1. Ultimately, 27 papers met the criteria for inclusion in the systematic review and meta-analysis. All included papers were longitudinal studies that examined the risk of dementia as their primary outcome and reported their results as HRs.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. PRISMA flow diagram.

3.2 Characteristics of included studies

Detailed characteristics of the included studies are presented in Tables 13. In total, 27 articles were included, with 10 focusing on early-life CR, 10 on middle-life CR, and 13 on late-life CR. The publication years of the included studies ranged from 2004 to 2023, with 70.4% published in the last decade and 44.4% in the last 5 years. The sample sizes of the individual studies varied widely, ranging from 176 to 1,341,842 participants. The proportion of females in studies including both sexes ranged from 29.7 to 76.0%, while four articles exclusively included males. The studies adjusted for multiple potential confounders, including age, sex, education, race/ethnicity, moderate drinking, current smoking, healthy diet score, obesity, diabetes, hypertension, depression, social isolation, hearing problems, regular physical activity, high cholesterol, cardiovascular disease, and traumatic brain injury, among others.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Detailed characteristics of studies on early-life CR and risk of dementia.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Detailed characteristics of studies on middle-life CR and risk of dementia.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Detailed characteristics of studies on late-life CR and risk of dementia.

3.3 Measurement of life course cognitive reserve

In the included studies, early-life CR proxies primarily consisted of education. Proxies for midlife CR included occupation complexity and social network contact, while proxies for CR in late-life encompassed cognitive activity and social connection. Given the diverse geographical origins of the study populations, variations in educational attainment classifications may exist, potentially leading to partial heterogeneity when combining results. To address this, the study classified the lowest level of education into two categories: primary and junior high school education. Participants with less than 7 years of education were categorized as having primary education, while those with less than 10 years were categorized as having junior high school education. This standardization facilitated subsequent meta-analysis. CR proxies were assessed using validated questionnaires in all studies, with the lowest level of each CR proxy indicator used as a reference category for comparison with the highest level. When the highest level was used as a reference category, the inverse of the HR and its corresponding 95% CI were utilized in the data analysis. Detailed measurements of life course CR are provided in Tables 13.

3.4 Diagnosis of dementia

All the included studies diagnosed dementia according to accepted standards, such as The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-III, DSM-III-R, DSM-IV, DSM-5) (Wakefield, 2016), International Classification of Diseases (ICD) (The Lancet, 2019), Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) (Morris, 1993), National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS), and Guidelines group of Alzheimer’s Disease Chinese (ADC) 2021.

3.5 Quality assessment and potential bias

The quality assessment results are presented in Table 4. The study quality scores ranged from 6 out of 9 to 9 out of 9. Among the 27 studies, 21 were assessed as having a low risk of bias, while 6 studies were categorized as having a moderate risk of bias. Notably, some gender-limited studies incurred a reduction in quality within the selection domain. Additionally, all studies utilized self-assessment of exposure, which contributed to reduced quality in the selection domain, except for one study (Dartigues et al., 2013) that employed structured interviews. Furthermore, given the longitudinal nature of the included studies, there was a certain risk of loss to follow-up, resulting in a lower score in the outcome domain. Specifically, 9 out of 10 studies analyzing early-life CR and the risk of dementia exhibited a low bias, while 8 out of 10 studies analyzing midlife CR and the risk of dementia demonstrated a low bias. Lastly, 10 out of 13 studies analyzing late-life CR and the risk of dementia had a low bias.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Assessment of quality and risk of bias according to the Newcastle-Ottawa scale.

The funnel plots depicting the association of early-life, middle-life, and late-life CR with the risk of dementia are presented in Supplementary Appendix B1. Results from the Begg and Egger tests indicated no evidence of publication bias for the early-life period (p = 0.858 and 0.757). However, a statistically significant difference was observed for the middle-life period (p = 0.049 and 0.099) and late-life period (p = 0.729 and 0.001), suggesting potential publication bias. Additionally, the trim-and-fill method was employed as a supplementary analysis, but no studies were filled, indicating the stability of the findings (Supplementary Appendix B5).

3.6 Study findings

All of the studies evaluated the association between longitudinal changes in CR and the risk of dementia.

3.6.1 Early-life CR and risk of dementia

A total of 10 articles reported associations between proxies of early-life CR and the risk of dementia. A random-effects meta-analysis of early-life CR and the risk of dementia was conducted and is presented in Figure 2. Overall, individuals with high early-life CR had an 18% lower risk of dementia compared to those with low early-life CR (HR: 0.82; 95% CI: 0.79–0.86). Heterogeneity (I2) was low at 12.3%. Subgroup analysis of different CR proxy indicators in early-life revealed that high scores on these early-life CR proxies, including education up to junior high school (HR: 0.81; 95% CI: 0.72–0.91) and education up to primary school (HR: 0.82; 95% CI: 0.81–0.83), were associated with a relatively lower risk of dementia compared to low scores. I2 ranged from 0 to 44.2%.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Association of early-life CR with risk of dementia in different proxies.

3.6.2 Middle-life CR and risk of dementia

A total of 10 studies reported associations between proxies of mid-life CR and the risk of dementia. A random-effects meta-analysis revealed that individuals with higher midlife CR tended to have a decreased risk of dementia (HR: 0.91; 95% CI: 0.84–0.98), as shown in Figure 3. I2 level was 0%. Subgroup analysis based on different mid-life CR proxies indicated that high social network contact (HR: 0.92; 95% CI: 0.83–1.01) and high occupational complexity (HR: 0.89; 95% CI: 0.78–1.01) may not be significantly associated with dementia risk.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Association of middle-life CR with risk of dementia in different proxies.

3.6.3 Late-life CR and risk of dementia

A total of 13 articles reported associations between proxies of late-life CR and the risk of dementia. Random-effects meta-analysis (Figure 4) illustrates these associations. The HR value was 0.81 (95% CI: 0.75–0.88), with an I2 level of 72.4%. All three proxies of cognitive activity and social connection exhibited statistically significant associations with the risk of dementia. The outcome of the meta-analysis indicated a subtotal HR of 0.91 (95% CI: 0.86–0.97) for cognitive activity and 0.70 (95% CI: 0.63–0.77) for social connection. I2 ranged from 0 to 55.4%, suggesting that CR proxies in late life may contribute to heterogeneity.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Association of late-life CR with risk of dementia in different proxies.

3.7 Sensitivity analyses

The results remained consistent even after switching from the random effects model to the fixed effects model in early life, middle life, and late-life (Supplementary Appendix D1). Similarly, consistency persisted when any single study was excluded from the meta-analyses of early-life, middle-life, and late-life (Supplementary Appendix D2). Subgroup analysis according to different proxies of CR also yielded consistent results (Figures 24). Although the Begg and Egger tests suggested potential publication bias in middle-life and late-life results, the trim-and-fill method did not add any studies, indicating the stability of the results (Supplementary Appendix B5).

4 Discussion

CR has been proposed as a compensatory mechanism to cope with age-related brain injury and to explain interindividual differences in the ability to maintain cognitive function in the presence of brain pathology (Xu et al., 2019). As individuals age, CR accumulates, and the proxies for each life stage may vary (Chapko et al., 2018). However, it remains unclear which life course stages and variables significantly influence dementia risk, with conflicting evidence supporting the association between CR and dementia risk (Takasugi et al., 2019; Wang et al., 2019; Zhang et al., 2021; Duffner et al., 2022). To address this gap in research, a meta-analysis was conducted examining the relationship between life-course CR and dementia risk.

4.1 CR proxy differences

In this systematic review and meta-analysis of 27 longitudinal studies, the association between CR and the risk of dementia over the life course (early, middle, and late life) was assessed.

4.1.1 Early-life CR

In the assessment of individuals with high early CR, an 18% decreased risk of dementia was observed compared to those with poor early CR, as indicated by proxies for CR. Upon categorizing different proxies, varying levels of education were observed to correlate with differing degrees of dementia risk reduction. Educational attainment, the most commonly utilized CR proxy for early life, was significantly associated with a reduced risk of dementia later in life in the meta-analysis results. Further subgroup analysis of primary and junior high school education revealed a more pronounced effect of higher education levels on dementia risk among individuals with primary school education compared to those with junior high school education. This disparity may stem from the increasingly intricate neural network connections and heightened plasticity associated with higher education levels, enhancing the brain’s resilience to pathological damage. Based on these findings, emphasis should be placed on education. This conclusion is consistent with the studies by Seblova et al. (2021) and Xu et al. (2016) that found that each additional year of education beyond the baseline reduced the risk of dementia in later life. Thus, it is recommended to prioritize primary and secondary education, even if universal university education remains unattainable, as it plays a crucial role in promoting cognitive health later in life.

4.1.2 Middle-life CR

Overall, mid-life CR accumulation was associated with a 9% decreased risk of dementia. Further subgroup analysis of CR proxies revealed varying effects on dementia risk. Specifically, a statistically significant association was observed between midlife occupational complexity and the risk of dementia. Occupational complexity serves as the most commonly used CR proxy indicator in midlife, providing the most accurate reflection of CR levels during this period. Consequently, its findings objectively depict the relationship between dementia risk and CR levels in middle age. The influence of occupational complexity on dementia onset may be realized through its complexity with people, things, and data. This suggests that different types of occupational complexity may have distinct associations with the risk of dementia. However, the limited research on the classification of occupational complexity necessitates further investigation. In a more thorough analysis, Kroeger et al. (2008) discovered that complexity with people or things, rather than data, was associated with a lower risk of dementia. Therefore, the various impacts of occupational complexity classification on dementia risk warrant further study.

The association between social contact and the risk of dementia in the current study was not significant. However, several studies (Hwang et al., 2023; Li et al., 2023) in older age found social contact to be significantly associated with the risk of dementia in later life. It was hypothesized that this absence of statistical significance was attributed to the limited literature on social contact during mid-life included in the current study. Sommerlad et al. (2019) found that although the association between social contact in mid-life and dementia risk was not statistically significant, more frequent social contact during midlife was associated with better subsequent cognitive trajectories. Therefore, considering that social isolation is a risk factor for dementia (Shen et al., 2022b), appropriate social contact in midlife remains necessary.

4.1.3 Late-life CR

In comparison to early and middle life, there are significantly more studies conducted in late life. This trend may be attributed to the fact that longitudinal studies commencing in early or middle age require longer durations, and researchers often prefer to follow up in later life due to the higher likelihood of dementia development. Moreover, studies conducted in late life tend to have larger study populations, thereby enhancing the validity of their findings. Overall, individuals with high levels of late-life CR exhibited a 19% reduced incidence of dementia compared to those with low levels. Subgroup analysis of the proxies revealed that cognitive activity had a mitigating effect on the risk of dementia, with notably significant differences observed. Additionally, social connection may exert a relatively superior protective impact on dementia risk.

The results of a subgroup meta-analysis focusing on late-life CR and dementia risk underscore the significant association between social connection and dementia risk. This finding is consistent with similar conclusions drawn from other studies. For instance, Evans et al. (2019) conducted a meta-analysis investigating social isolation and cognitive function in later life, revealing that lower levels of social isolation, indicative of higher social engagement and connection, were linked to better cognitive function in later years. Notably, social loneliness and isolation are more prevalent among older adults compared to younger age groups. Shen et al. (2022a) further supported these findings, suggesting that social isolation might serve as an early indicator of heightened dementia risk, thereby advocating for early dementia prevention strategies. While there is currently no standardized international intervention (Fakoya et al., 2020), enhancing social networks and promoting increased social interaction among the elderly could represent a promising avenue for dementia prevention efforts.

Moreover, the current study revealed that engaging in cognitive activities such as reading, solving puzzles, and playing chess, and card games during later life was associated with a modest reduction in dementia risk. Notably, cognitive activity is classified as level A evidence for evidence-based prevention of Alzheimer’s disease (Yu et al., 2020). As individuals age, cognitively stimulating activities often decrease due to retirement and declines in physical function. However, maintaining cognitive activity in late life could help preserve the integrity of the brain’s white matter (Palta et al., 2021), potentially contributing to the observed association with reduced dementia risk.

4.2 Period differences

Overall, concerning different life stages, the protective effect of CR appears to be similar. Our findings indicate that CR in early and late life may confer a slightly higher protective effect against dementia risk compared to CR in middle life. This observation could be attributed to the fact that dementia typically manifests in later life, and the impact of CR proxies may be more pronounced when cognitive function has declined, especially compared to the cognitively normal midlife period. Alty et al. (2023) also suggested that CR interventions in old age may offer greater benefits, particularly for individuals with lower levels of education. This could be particularly encouraging for older males who may not have attained higher levels of education in their early years due to various reasons. Moreover, early-life CR accumulation demonstrated a more substantial protective effect, suggesting that while increasing CR at any stage is beneficial, accumulating CR earlier in life may offer greater risk reduction for dementia. This aligns with the findings of Wang et al. (2017), who proposed that higher exposure to CR proxies is associated with a reduced risk of dementia, highlighting the potential benefits of interventions initiated earlier in life.

4.3 Strengths and limitations of the study

In this review, the included studies were all longitudinal, providing a systematic and detailed understanding of the continuous developmental process and the laws of change. All eligible studies were published after 2004, with 70.4% of articles published in the last decade and 44.4% in the last 5 years, indicating the recent surge in researchers’ interest in the relationship between CR and dementia. The quality of the literature was assessed using internationally accepted standards, and all included studies were of medium to low-risk quality. Additionally, various sensitivity analyses were conducted to explore sources of heterogeneity, ensuring the stability and reliability of the experimental results.

Some limitations existed in the current study. For instance, although the quality of the literature was assessed using the NOS scale, since all studies measured CR in the form of questionnaires, the data reported by the subjects may have been somewhat biased from the actual situation and also led to some possible survivor bias. Additionally, various sensitivity analyses were employed to analyze the stability of the results, considering that the cohorts included in the studies were all healthy person cohorts, the effect of healthy cohort bias still needed to be considered. Studies came from different countries and regions, and some used different questionnaires, but the general direction was similar, and efforts were made to reduce such heterogeneity when screening the literature. Furthermore, Begg and Egger’s test indicated potential publication bias in studies in middle and late life. Nonetheless, the trim-and-fill method was conducted, and the results indicated stable conclusions. The I2 for late life was 72.4%, suggesting considerable heterogeneity, possibly because some studies reported results that were not expressed as HR and were therefore not included in the meta-analysis. To investigate the impact of late-life heterogeneity on the results of this study’s meta-analysis, several sensitivity analyses were conducted. These analyses demonstrated that the results of the meta-analysis remained unaffected, indicating the reliability of the conclusions.

Some CR proxies might not have been measured at every stage of the life course, potentially leading to missing indicators such as late-life occupation. This issue is also related to the insufficient number of existing studies, which could be updated if enough research on late-life occupation and dementia risk emerges in the future. Additionally, since most of the included studies initiated follow-up in late life, some data on the incidence of dementia in young and middle-aged individuals might have been inadequate. Limited by the amount of literature that met the criteria, this study focused on overall dementia risk. In the future, if possible, dementia could be typed, e.g., early-onset dementia and late-onset dementia, which may provide some recommendations and a basis for intervention in dementia prevention in the younger population. Considering the conceptual rigor of CR, this study did not include physical activity in the theoretical framework of the literature search. However, based on the available research (WHO, 2019), it is evident that exercise has many beneficial effects on brain health, helps reduce the risk of dementia, depression and stress, and plays a role in restoring and maintaining cognitive function and metabolic control, which is a strong protective factor against cognitive decline and dementia. Little is known about the mechanisms driving these effects, and future research could further investigate the relationship between CR, physical activity and dementia. In addition, due to the limitation of the number of available studies, meta-analysis of indicators determined by multiple factors (e.g., socioeconomic status) was not performed in this review. Referring to existing studies (Al Hazzouri et al., 2011), cumulative socioeconomic status also has a protective effect on dementia risk. Future studies could explore the relationship between indicators determined by multifaceted factors and dementia risk from the perspective of socioeconomic status. Unfortunately, given that little is known about the shared or separate underlying mechanisms in CR proxies, and given the differences in levels and variations in the effects of CR proxies on dementia risk, the present study cannot uncritically propose relevant interventions. This complexity is far from resolved and provides interesting directions for future work.

5 Conclusion

When synthesizing the findings from studies on CR and dementia risk across the life course, significant effects were observed for various CR proxies. These proxies included education in early life, occupation, social network contact in middle life, and cognitive activity and social connection in late life. The impact of CR proxies in early life and late life appeared to be more pronounced compared to middle life. Among these proxies, social connection emerged as potentially a effective approach for reducing dementia risk. However, given the impact of level versus change effects, further research is needed to determine which CR proxies to use at which stage to intervene in patients at risk for dementia.

Author contributions

YL: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Software, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. GL: Data curation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. LL: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. YH: Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. WG: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Project administration, Software, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnagi.2024.1358992/full#supplementary-material

References

Al Hazzouri, A. Z., Haan, M. N., Kalbfleisch, J. D., Galea, S., Lisabeth, L. D., and Aiello, A. E. (2011). Life-course socioeconomic position and incidence of dementia and cognitive impairment without dementia in older Mexican Americans: results from the Sacramento area Latino study on aging. Am. J. Epidemiol. 173, 1148–1158. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwq483

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Alty, J. E., Bindoff, A. D., Stuart, K. E., Roccati, E., Collins, J. M., King, A. E., et al. (2023). Sex-specific protective effects of cognitive reserve on age-related cognitive decline: a 5-year prospective cohort study. Neurology 100, e211–e219. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000201369

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Alzheimer’s Association (2022). 2022 Alzheimer's disease facts and figures. Alzheimers Dement. 18, 700–789. doi: 10.1002/alz.12638

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Aranda, M. P., Kremer, I. N., Hinton, L., Zissimopoulos, J., Whitmer, R. A., Hummel, C. H., et al. (2021). Impact of dementia: health disparities, population trends, care interventions, and economic costs. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 69, 1774–1783. doi: 10.1111/jgs.17345

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P., and Rothstein, H. R. (2010). A basic introduction to fixed-effect and random-effects models for meta-analysis. Res. Synth. Methods 1, 97–111. doi: 10.1002/jrsm.12

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chapko, D., McCormack, R., Black, C., and R. StaffMurray, A. (2018). Life-course determinants of cognitive reserve (CR) in cognitive aging and dementia - a systematic literature review. Aging Ment. Health 22, 915–926. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2017.1348471

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dartigues, J. F., Foubert-Samier, A., Le Goff, M., Viltard, M., Amieva, H., Orgogozo, J. M., et al. (2013). Playing board games, cognitive decline and dementia: a French population-based cohort study. BMJ Open 3:e002998. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2013-002998

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dekhtyar, S., Wang, H.-X., Fratiglioni, L., and Herlitz, A. (2016). Childhood school performance, education and occupational complexity: a life-course study of dementia in the Kungsholmen project. Int. J. Epidemiol. 45, 1207–1215. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyw008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ding, D., Zhao, Q., Wu, W., Xiao, Z., Liang, X., Luo, J., et al. (2020). Prevalence and incidence of dementia in an older Chinese population over two decades: the role of education. Alzheimers Dement. 16, 1650–1662. doi: 10.1002/alz.12159

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Duffner, L. A., Deckers, K., Cadar, D., Steptoe, A., de Vugt, M., and Köhler, S. (2022). The role of cognitive and social leisure activities in dementia risk: assessing longitudinal associations of modifiable and non-modifiable risk factors. Epidemiol. Psychiatr. Sci. 31:e5. doi: 10.1017/S204579602100069X

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, I. E. M., Martyr, A., Collins, R., Brayne, C., and Clare, L. (2019). Social isolation and cognitive function in later life: a systematic review and Meta-analysis. J. Alzheimers Dis. 70, S119–S144. doi: 10.3233/JAD-180501

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fakoya, O. A., McCorry, N. K., and Donnelly, M. (2020). Loneliness and social isolation interventions for older adults: a scoping review of reviews. BMC Public Health 20:129. doi: 10.1186/s12889-020-8251-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Foverskov, E., Glymour, M. M., Mortensen, E. L., Osler, M., Okholm, G. T., and Lund, R. (2020). Education and adolescent cognitive ability as predictors of dementia in a cohort of Danish men. PLoS One 15:e0235781. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0235781

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hwang, P. H., Ang, T. F. A., De Anda-Duran, I., Liu, X., Liu, Y., Gurnani, A., et al. (2023). Examination of potentially modifiable dementia risk factors across the adult life course: the Framingham heart study. Alzheimers Dement. 19, 2975–2983. doi: 10.1002/alz.12940

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hyun, J., Hall, C. B., Katz, M. J., Derby, C. A., Lipnicki, D. M., Crawford, J. D., et al. (2022). Education, occupational complexity, and incident dementia: a COSMIC collaborative cohort study. J. Alzheimers Dis. 85, 179–196. doi: 10.3233/JAD-210627

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jonathan, J. P. H., Deeks, J., and Altman, D. G. (2022). “Chapter 10: Analysing data and undertaking meta-analyses” in Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions version 62. eds. T. J. Higgins JPT, J. Chandler, M. Cumpston, T. Li, M. J. Page, and V. A. Welch.

Google Scholar

Kroeger, E., Andel, R., Lindsay, J., Benounissa, Z., Verreault, R., and Laurin, D. (2008). Is complexity of work associated with risk of dementia? Am. J. Epidemiol. 167, 820–830. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwm382

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Y., Yang, Y., Zhao, P., Wang, J., Mi, B., Zhao, Y., et al. (2023). Longitudinal associations between specific types/amounts social contact and cognitive function among middle-aged and elderly Chinese: a causal inference and longitudinal targeted maximum likelihood estimation analysis. J. Affect. Disord. 331, 158–166. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2023.03.039

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Livingston, G., Huntley, J., Sommerlad, A., Ames, D., Ballard, C., Banerjee, S., et al. (2020). Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the lancet commission. Lancet 396, 413–446. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30367-6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, J. C. (1993). The clinical dementia rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurology 43, 2412–2414. doi: 10.1212/WNL.43.11.2412-a

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Najar, J., Östling, S., Gudmundsson, P., Sundh, V., Johansson, L., Kern, S., et al. (2019). Cognitive and physical activity and dementia: a 44-year longitudinal population study of women. Neurology 92, e1322–e1330. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000007021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Page, M. J., McKenzie, J. E., Bossuyt, P. M., Boutron, I., Hoffmann, T. C., Mulrow, C. D., et al. (2021). The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 372:n71. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n71

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Palta, P., Gabriel, K. P., Kumar, A., Sharrett, A. R., Evenson, K. R., Gottesman, R. F., et al. (2021). Sedentary behavior in mid-life and risk of change in global cognitive function and incident dementia: the atherosclerosis risk in communities neurocognitive study (Aric-Ncs). Circulation 143:AP149. doi: 10.1161/circ.143.suppl_1.P149

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Palta, P., Sharrett, A. R., Deal, J. A., Evenson, K. R., Gabriel, K. P., Folsom, A. R., et al. (2019). Leisure-time physical activity sustained since midlife and preservation of cognitive function: the atherosclerosis risk in communities study. Alzheimers Dement. 15, 273–281. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.08.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Prince, M., Acosta, D., Ferri, C. P., Guerra, M., Huang, Y., Llibre Rodriguez, J. J., et al. (2012). Dementia incidence and mortality in middle-income countries, and associations with indicators of cognitive reserve: a 10/66 dementia research group population-based cohort study. Lancet 380, 50–58. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60399-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reas, E. T., Laughlin, G. A., Bergstrom, J., Kritz-Silverstein, D., Richard, E. L., Barrett-Connor, E., et al. (2019). Lifetime physical activity and late-life cognitive function: the rancho Bernardo study. Age Ageing 48, 241–246. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afy188

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text |

留言 (0)

沒有登入
gif