Mathematical performance in childhood and early adult outcomes after very preterm birth: an individual participant data meta‐analysis

Preterm birth has life-course consequences, in particular for the smallest infants and those born very preterm. Globally, about 1% to 2% of all infants are born at less than 32 weeks' gestation every year.1 Improved neonatal care has led to increased survival rates for these infants,2 but the high prevalence of cognitive and learning problems in surviving infants has not substantially changed over the last two decades.3, 4 Children born very preterm (<32wks' gestation or <1500g birthweight) have an increased risk for poorer academic attainment than their term-born peers, especially in mathematics.5-8 This risk persists into adolescence9 and may diminish life-long opportunities, contributing to the frequently observed lower rates of postsecondary education and higher rates of unemployment in preterm cohorts.10 Longitudinal outcome studies in adulthood consistently show that problems of cognition, neurodevelopment, and academic achievement continue over the life-course.11-15 In addition to single cohort studies and literature-based reviews, two studies have documented considerable temporal and cross-national consistency in cognitive and mathematics performance among children born very preterm.16, 17 However, it is unclear whether difficulties in mathematics among children born very preterm are accounted for by low IQ5 or not.7, 18

Among general population studies, mathematics skills in childhood have independent positive effects on adult economic success.19, 20 However, individuals born very preterm often experience severe medical complications and subsequent neurocognitive difficulties.21-23 Accordingly, studies in children and adults born very preterm have documented a higher prevalence of deficits in general cognitive performance in conjunction with poorer mathematics attainment than in term-born controls.24 One prospective longitudinal study of individuals born very preterm to 26 years of age investigated the extent to which mathematics performance in childhood explained adult economic success, but found no direct nor indirect effects after controlling for IQ.25 However, limitations of many follow-up studies into adulthood include attrition and relatively small samples, reducing statistical power.

Over and above IQ and mathematics performance, maternal education/family socioeconomic status is associated with long-term educational attainment, employment success, and wealth in adulthood.10, 20, 26, 27 In addition, general population studies consistently report that females outperform males in school.28 For example, the life-course Panel Study of Income Dynamics found that females, on average, completed more years of schooling26 while males, on average, had higher annual incomes.26 Evidence for sex differences in life-course trajectories among individuals born very preterm is still limited, but there is emerging evidence of males being more affected by preterm birth than females with regard to educational attainment.29

In this study we aimed to determine the strength of the independent associations between mathematics performance in children born very preterm and two adult outcomes—postsecondary education and being employed or in education—while adjusting for year of birth, gestational age, sex, maternal education, and child IQ.

1 METHOD

This study was registered as an individual participant data (IPD) meta-analysis with the International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews PROSPERO (protocol #CRD42020175032).

1.1 Search strategy and study selection criteria

A two-step process was applied to identify and select studies for inclusion. First, members of the Adults born Preterm International Collaboration (APIC; www.apic-preterm.org) were presented with the study protocol. APIC is a collaborative group of international researchers who study life-course outcomes of individuals born very preterm. All APIC members were invited to contribute data to this study. Prospective longitudinal studies that had assessed participants born very preterm with standardized intelligence and mathematics tests in middle childhood and with adult data on postsecondary education and employment/education status were eligible for inclusion. Only studies with data available from at least three timepoints (i.e., birth, childhood, and adulthood) that included a contemporaneous comparison group of children born at term for the childhood assessments were included in the data harmonization and pooling process. There was no restriction for year of birth. A total of six APIC cohort studies were identified through this process by June 2019. Second, a systematic literature search was conducted in English in PubMed and PsycINFO using the keywords ‘very low birth weight’ OR (‘very preterm’ OR ‘premature*’ OR ‘gestation*’) AND (‘math*’ OR ‘arithmetic’) AND (‘intelligence’ OR ‘IQ’ OR ‘cognitive’) AND (‘adult*’). Screening and review of articles did not identify additional data sets that fulfilled the eligibility criteria. Accordingly, data from the following six birth cohorts were included in this study (ordered alphabetically by country).

1.1.1 Australia

The Victorian Infant Collaborative Study included all livebirths less than 1000g and/or less than 28 weeks' gestational age in the state of Victoria in 1991 to 1992 and a normal birthweight (>2500g) term comparison group recruited in the newborn period.3 At 8 years of age, mathematics performance was assessed using the Wide Range Achievement Test, Third Edition, Arithmetic scale,30 and IQ using the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Third Edition.31 Adult assessments were carried out at 18 and 25 years of age.32

1.1.2 Canada

The McMaster University Study included all livebirths weighing 1000g or less in the Central-West Ontario region from 1977 to 1982, and a normal birthweight term comparison group recruited at school age.33 At 8 years of age, children were evaluated using the Wide Range Achievement Test Arithmetic scale30 and the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children – Revised.34 Adult assessments were carried out at 29 years of age.14, 35

1.1.3 Germany

The Bavarian Longitudinal Study included all livebirths born very preterm in Southern Bavaria in 1985 and 1986 and a normal birthweight term comparison group.36 At 8 years of age, children were evaluated using a standardized Mathematic Test5, 37 and the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children Mental Processing Composite scale (IQ-equivalent).38 Adult assessments were carried out at 26 years of age.39

1.1.4 UK and Republic of Ireland

The EPICure study is a nationwide study comprising all infants born at 22 to 25 completed weeks of gestation in the UK and Republic of Ireland between March and December 1995, and a term-born comparison group of peers recruited at school age.40, 41 At 11 years of age, children were evaluated using the Wechsler Individual Achievement Test Second UK Edition Mathematics Composite scale42 and the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children Mental Processing Composite scale (IQ).38 Adult assessments were carried out at 19 years of age.43

1.1.5 USA

The Case Western Reserve University Cohort Study included all infants with birthweights less than 1500g admitted to Rainbow Babies and Children's Hospital in Cleveland from 1977 to 1979 and a normal birthweight comparison group recruited at age 8 years.44 At 8 years of age, children were evaluated using the Woodcock-Johnson Achievement Test Mathematics Scale45 and the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children – Revised.34 Adult assessments were carried out at 20 years of age.44

1.1.6 USA

The Rhode Island Cohort of Adults Born Preterm Study included livebirths weighing less than 1500g in the specialty hospital from 1985 to 1989 and a ‘healthy’ term-born comparison group.46, 47 At 8 years of age, children were evaluated using the Wide Range Achievement Test, Third Edition, Arithmetic scale30 and the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Third Edition.31 Adult assessments were carried out at 23 years of age.48

1.2 Data extraction, harmonization, and analysis

Participating groups (n=6) obtained ethical permission and completed a data sharing agreement with the University of Tennessee Knoxville to transfer non-identifiable individual level data for the analysis. Groups were requested to provide neonatal data (i.e., infant gestational age at birth [wks], birthweight [g], sex [binary coded], maternal education), childhood IQ and mathematics test results (standard scores), and adult employment and educational attainment information.

Data were harmonized in SPSS v24 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA), including a variable for each cohort's year of birth. The content of the IQ and mathematics tests (e.g., dimensions assessed) were compared to confirm that these generally targeted very similar domains (see Table 1, online supporting information, for details). Test scores were z-standardized according to each cohort-specific contemporaneous comparison group before pooling. In line with an adaptive approach to scoring test performance, participants with IQ scores more than 2 SD below the control participants' mean (i.e., <−2 SD) were coded as having intellectual impairment. Information about maternal education was recoded into an interval-scaled 9-level variable according to the International Standard Classification of Education.49 The two dependent variables in young adulthood (i.e., 19–29y) were binary-coded as ‘any postsecondary education’ (1=yes, ≥International Standard Classification of Education level 5 and 0=no, <International Standard Classification of Education level 5) and ‘currently employed or in education’ (1=yes/0=no).

One-stage IPD meta-analyses were performed as mixed effects logistic regressions in Stata 16 (StataCorp, College Station, TX, USA). All models were controlled for fixed effects of cohort year of birth, infant gestational age at birth, sex, maternal education, and childhood IQ, as well as the nestedness of data in cohorts (i.e., including a random effect for study site). The fixed and random effects of childhood mathematics scores on postsecondary education and being employed/in education in young adulthood are reported. Including random effects allowed for different relationships with adult outcomes within cohorts. As part of a stepwise process, random effects of other covariates were additionally included, and model fit was evaluated using log-likelihood goodness of fit tests. Random effects were retained if their inclusion significantly improved overall model fit. Specifically, with regard to attending postsecondary education, the additional inclusion of random effects for gestation and IQ improved model fit, indicated by a significant log-likelihood ratio χ2 test (χ2[4]=26.30, p<0.001), fixed effects remained stable. With regard to being employed/in education, the additional inclusion of other covariates' random effects did not improve model fit, indicated by a non-significant log-likelihood ratio χ2 test (χ2[4]=9.11, p=0.058).

2 RESULTS

Individual cohort sample sizes ranged from 117 to 231 participants (Table 1). Sex was equally distributed across study cohorts but there was substantial variation with regard to gestational age and birthweight, because of different cohort inclusion criteria, and in distributions of maternal education levels. Mean childhood IQ and mathematics z-scores ranged from −1.57 to −0.36 and − 1.55 to −0.51, respectively. Across cohorts there was variation in the proportion of adults who had attended postsecondary education (18–57%) and in those employed or in education at the time of assessment (71–88%).

Table 1. Descriptive characteristics by included cohort Australia (Victoria; n=166) Canada (McMaster; n=122) Germany (BLS; n=201) UK (EPICure; n=117) USA (Cleveland; n=231) USA (Rhode Island; n=117) Recruitment criteria <28wks GA/<1000g BW <1001g BW <32wks GA/<1500g BW 22–25wks GA <1500g BW <1500g BW Year(s) of birth 1991–2 1977–82 1985–6 1995 1977–9 1985–9 Female 55.4% 53.3% 46.8% 55.6% 51.5% 55.6% Birthweight (g) 885 (155) 841 (121) 1341 (324) 742 (126) 1177 (219) 1251 (329) Gestation (wks) 26.7 (2.0) 27.0 (2.2) 30.6 (2.2) 24.4 (0.76) 30.2 (2.3) 30.0 (2.55) Mother's educationa 3.15 (0.87) 3.12 (1.26) 3.02 (1.08) 2.99 (0.99) 2.92 (0.74) 4.03 (1.12) Child assessment age 8y 8y 8y 11y 8y 8y Childhood mathb –0.62 (1.05) –0.90 (1.23) –0.74 (1.15) –1.55 (1.35) –0.51 (1.46) –0.57 (1.03) Childhood IQb –0.51 (1.14) –0.95 (1.24) –0.72 (1.33) –1.57 (1.44) –0.36 (1.15) –0.59 (1.28) IQ <–2SDb 8.4% 18.9% 12.9% 35.0% 0.0% 12.0% Adult assessment age 25y 29y 26y 19y 20y 23y Employed/in education 78.3% 77.0% 87.6% 85.5% 80.5% 70.9% Postsecondary educationc 56.6% of n=143 39.1% of n=64 17.9% of n=201 25.2% of n=111 36.4% of n=231 38.8% of n=116 2.1 One-stage IPD meta-analyses

Mixed effects logistic regression indicated substantial heterogeneity between study cohorts (i.e., significant log-likelihood χ2 tests), supporting the use of mixed effects models as opposed to logistic models without random effects.

Table 2 shows that adults born very preterm with higher mathematics test scores in childhood had higher odds to have attended any postsecondary education (fixed effect odds ratio [OR] per SD increase in mathematics test scores: 1.36 [95% confidence interval : 1.03, 1.79]). The random effect of mathematics was estimated at SD 0.21 (95% CI: 0.05, 0.82), indicating significant variation between cohorts in the relationship between these two variables. In addition, adults born very preterm with higher childhood IQ (OR per SD increase in IQ: 1.58 [95% CI: 1.26, 1.97]), higher educated mothers (OR per SD increase in maternal education: 1.29 [95% CI: 1.09, 1.53]), and females (OR: 2.00 [95% CI: 1.43, 2.78]) had higher odds to have attended postsecondary education.

Table 2. Multilevel mixed-effects model results (one-stage individual participant data) showing associations of childhood mathematics scores and other covariates with attending postsecondary education and being employed/in education in young adulthood in six very preterm cohorts Dependent variable

Postsecondary education (n=866)

OR (95% CI)

Employed/in education (n=954)

OR (95% CI)

Fixed effects Mathematics (per SD) 1.36 (1.03, 1.79)a 1.14 (0.87, 1.48) Year of birth (per year) 1.05 (0.97, 1.13) 1.00 (0.96, 1.03) Gestation (per week) 1.06 (0.98, 1.15) 0.97 (0.90, 1.03) Female 2.00 (1.43, 2.78)c 0.83 (0.59, 1.16) Mother's education (per point) 1.29 (1.09, 1.53)b 1.14 (0.96, 1.35) Participant IQ (per SD) 1.58 (1.26, 1.97)c 1.28 (1.06, 1.56)b Random effects by cohort SD (Mathematics) 0.21 (0.05, 0.82) 0.21 (0.09, 0.51) SD (Gestation) 0.02 (0.01, 0.04) _ SD (IQ) 0.10 (0.00, 4.11) _ Log-likelihood –455.04 –450.93 For fixed effects: ap<0.05; bp<0.01; cp<0.001. OR, odds ratio; CI, confidence interval; SD, standard deviation.

The fixed effect of childhood mathematics test scores (OR=1.14 [95% CI: 0.87, 1.48]) on current employment/education status was not significant, while the random effect was estimated at SD 0.21 (95% CI: 0.09, 0.51), indicating significant variation in this relationship between cohorts. Adults with higher childhood IQ were more likely to be employed or in education at follow-up (OR=1.28 [95% CI: 1.06, 1.56]). Figure 1 displays forest plots of the effects of childhood mathematics scores on postsecondary education and current employment/education status by cohort.

image

Forest plots showing one-stage individual participant data meta-analysis effects of childhood mathematics scores (odds ratios [ORs] and 95% confidence intervals [CIs]) on having attended postsecondary education and being employed/in education in young adulthood. Weights reflect individual study n while accounting for within-study variability and between-study variance. BLS, Bavarian Longitudinal Study; ML, maximum likelihood.

As sensitivity analyses, calculations were repeated three times. First, to test the effects of mathematics test scores on adult outcomes without including control variables. These showed similar but stronger associations of mathematics performance with both postsecondary education and current employment/education status (see Table S2, online supporting information). Second, because 118 participants had intellectual impairment, models were rerun after their exclusion, again with similar findings (see Table S3, online supporting information). Finally, considering potential outlier effects of the EPICure study data indicated in Figure 1, analyses were rerun without these 117 EPICure participants. In these models, the fixed effect of childhood mathematics test scores on attending postsecondary education (OR=1.18 [95% CI: 0.87, 1.61]) was not significant, nor was the effect on current employment/education status (OR=1.11 [95% CI: 0.90, 1.38]).

3 DISCUSSION

In this IPD meta-analysis of six very preterm cohort studies from five different countries in Europe, North America, and Australia, born between 1977 and 1995, we found evidence that higher mathematics scores in childhood were independently associated with attending any postsecondary education. Evidence was weak for an independent association between mathematics performance and being employed or in education in young adulthood. The random effects of mathematics on attending postsecondary education and on being employed or in education were both significant, indicating substantial heterogeneity between study cohorts with regard to the relationship of mathematics with adult outcomes. Over and above the effects of mathematics, childhood IQ was independently associated with attending any postsecondary education and being employed or in education. Moreover, females born very preterm and those with higher educated mothers at birth were more likely to attend postsecondary education.

It is important to highlight that this study did not investigate effects of very preterm birth as has traditionally been done in similar meta-analyses. The cohort-specific contemporaneous control data were only used to standardize childhood test scores. With regard to the other variables in the mixed effects models, adults born very preterm who had been born to higher educated mothers had 29% higher odds of attending postsecondary education, as previously indicated in other studies.10, 39 The current findings confirm that educational and employment opportunities are enhanced with birth to higher educated mothers, no matter whether in Australia, Europe, or North America. Females born very preterm were twice as likely than males born very preterm to attend postsecondary education. This confirmation that males born very preterm have lower educational attainment than females across these different industrialized societies is important, and, in keeping with previous findings,26, 28, 29 suggests that males born very preterm may benefit from screening and targeted educational support. More research is needed to investigate why males born very preterm may do particularly poorly in education. Although not novel findings, the cross-national and temporal consistency of effects of maternal education and child sex found here may warrant renewed consideration for how early follow-up services and educational policies could support equal opportunities for all members of society. Moreover, our finding of heterogeneity between study cohorts with regard to the relationship of mathematics with adult outcomes may be further investigated by future studies. For instance, it may be worth assessing whether specific educational supports or therapies provided to some cohorts, or individuals within cohorts, might explain this variation and thereby point towards novel interventions.

Our models did not show fixed effects of gestational age at birth. However, variations in adult outcomes according to gestation may have been restricted by the limited range in gestation, mediated by functional childhood indicators such as IQ and mathematics performance, and they may also have been masked by the random effects of cohort membership, since each study cohort had applied different recruitment and sampling criteria, causing wide variations in gestation and birthweight between cohorts (Table 1).3, 33, 40, 44, 46, 50 Accordingly, Table 2 shows a significant random effect of gestation, indicating variation between cohorts in the relationship between gestational age and postsecondary education. Sensitivity analyses excluding EPICure study data also suggested that the association between mathematics performance and postsecondary education varied by gestational age, being stronger among participants born extremely preterm, whose average childhood performance may also have been comparably lower compared with term-born peers due to their birth on the lower extreme of the gestational age spectrum (see Table 1).

3.1 Strengths and limitations

Our IPD meta-analysis confirms our ability to predict life-course outcomes into early adulthood across different very preterm cohorts sampled across different birth epochs, neonatal health care services, and education systems. Using pooled IPD provided a large sample size and allowed better adjustment for confounding bias than a standard meta-analysis. Data harmonization and analysis protocols returned stable and reliable models that accounted for the heterogeneity between cohorts. In the future, we support recommendations to harmonize follow-up instruments51 to further facilitate comparative studies.

For reasons of ecological validity and data distribution we decided to use postsecondary education (i.e., having attended college after graduation from secondary school) as the dependent variable for educational attainment. Access to higher education has been found to represent an excellent indicator of attainment and economic success in adults born preterm.10 Although education data were recoded using International Standard Classification of Education levels,49 some structural differences between education systems may not be fully eliminated. For instance, graduating from high school and choosing the respective pathway into higher education may have a higher threshold in Germany and the UK than in North America, in part because alternative professional apprenticeship qualification pathways may be more typical for large parts of the population in Europe. For instance, according to 2020 OECD data,52 population rates of postsecondary educational attainment among 25- to 34-years-olds differ between Germany (35%) and Australia (54%) and the USA (52%). Respectively, the values in Table 1 should be interpreted with these country-specific differences in mind. Nevertheless, our main results focus on associations between variables, not descriptive rates of outcomes. Moreover, national differences in elementary school education (e.g., regular age at formal school entry, rates of delayed school entry, and rates of inclusion of children with disabilities [often very preterm] into mainstream schooling)17, 53 and differences in ages at assessments may have confounded the distribution of variables included in our models. In addition, the childhood assessment was administered at age 11 years in the EPICure cohort, and at age 8 years in the other five cohorts, which may have affected cohort participants' average performance in comparison to their age-matched controls. Indeed, our sensitivity analysis confirmed a slight variation in findings when excluding EPICure participants. At the time of the adult assessment, some cohorts' participants were still in secondary education and were excluded, therefore we had a slightly lower sample size for that dependent variable. In addition, data collection for the young adult assessments was conducted between ages 19 to 29 years, before some participants had fully completed their postsecondary education and established career trajectories. Accordingly, our assessment of the role of childhood mathematics performance for the socioeconomic success of adults born very preterm is limited to young adulthood; the findings require replication in older populations with participants in their 30s or 40s.

There is some risk of bias. Our literature search did not identify eligible cohorts beyond the APIC network, which was likely because of the complexity and strict criteria of our research question and resulting search term combinations, we did not search grey literature or trials in progress data. The search was performed in English only. Although unlikely, eligible cohorts whose data were simply not published within a life-course analysis framework may exist. Each of the co-authors of this current study were involved in at least one of the original cohort publications, and a formal investigation of the original data for risk of bias issues was not performed. Finally, despite spanning three continents, all cohorts included were from high-income countries, a limitation of most very preterm life-course studies.15, 54

4 CONCLUSIONS

Among infants born very preterm, performance in mathematics in childhood represents an independent functional indicator of attending postsecondary education, but it is not associated with being employed or in full-time education in early adulthood. IQ was strongly and consistently associated with both postsecondary education and employment/education in young adulthood. Despite substantial variations between cohorts (i.e., world regions and birth epochs), males born very preterm were consistently at higher risk than females born very preterm of not attending postsecondary education, as were infants born very preterm to mothers of lower educational background. These findings have implications for the planning of long-term follow-up and support after very preterm birth.

Acknowledgements

We would like to sincerely thank the APIC community for their feedback on the initial data harmonization and analysis strategy. Special thanks are due to Prof Eero Kajantie and Dr Petteri Hovi for allowing access to the Cleveland Study data set after Prof Maureen Hack's passing, and for sharing their harmonization documentation and legal agreement templates. JJ was supported by grant JA 1913/2-2 from the German Research Foundation. DW and PB were supported by an EU Horizon 2020 grant 733280 (RECAP-preterm) and DW by the New Opportunities for Research Funding Agency Co-operation in Europe, Dynamics of Inequality Across the Life-course Program (grant number: 462-16-040). The EPICure studies were funded by the Medical Research Council (MR/N024869/1). NM receives funding from the NIHR Biomedical Research Centre at University College London/University College London Hospitals. The McMaster Study was supported by grant No. ESPM85-201, Hospital for Sick Children Foundation, Toronto, Ontario and a Team Grant (2009H00529) from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research awarded to LS and SS. The Rhode Island cohort studies were funded by the National Institutes of Health, National Institute of Nursing Research (Grant # R01 NR 003695-01; R01 NR003695-14). PA was supported by the Australian National Health and Medical Research Council Investigator Grant (#1176077). JC was supported by Australian Medical Research Future Fund Career Development Fellowship (#1141354). The Victorian Infant Collaborative Study studies were funded by the Australian NHMRC (#1104300, #491246).

留言 (0)

沒有登入
gif