Roger W. Sperry (1913–1994)

Roger Wolcott Sperry made titanic contributions to neurobiology [1]. He did so by challenging some of the existing scientific dogmas and making discoveries that revolutionised the fields of neural development and hemispheric specialisation [2], Fig. 1. Sperry was born on the 20th of August 1913 in Hartford, Connecticut, and spent his early years on a nearby farm with his family [3]. His father, Francis Bushnell, worked in the banking business, but died when Roger was 11 years old [4]. After this, Roger’s mother, Florence Kraemer Sperry, relocated with the family to West Hartford and started to work as an assistant to the principal in a high school to look after Roger and his 1-year younger brother, Russel Loomis [4]. At Hall High School, Sperry showed his academic talent and excelled in several sports—he even set a state record in the javelin throw [2]. He won an Amos C. Miller Scholarship to join Oberlin College, where a Sperry Neuroscience Wing in the Kettering Hall building was inaugurated in 1990. At Oberlin College, he was the captain of the basketball team and continued to excel in sports [3]. Over the years, he matured a curiosity in palaeontology, and, in his free time, he was a painter, sculptor and ceramist.

Fig. 1figure 1

Portrait of Roger Sperry. Credits: National Library of Medicine, Digital Collections. Courtesy of the National Library of Medicine

But at the core of his interests there was psychology, a passion reportedly spurred by an introductory module on psychology given by R. H. Stetson, a leading authority on the physiology of speech [2] who studied at Harvard with William James [5]. During his psychology course as a freshman, Sperry wrote the following questions on the first page of his class notes: ‘Where does behaviour come from? What is the purpose of consciousness?’ [3, 5]. In many ways, these questions shaped his research activities for the coming years. Sperry completed his undergraduate degree in English (1935) before obtaining his master’s degree in psychology (1937). He then completed his PhD in zoology at the University of Chicago in 1941 with neuroembryologist Paul A. Weiss, and, between 1942 and 1945, he also performed ‘military service on the Medical Research Project on Nerve Injuries’ [6]. His experiments continued with Karl Lashley at Harvard and the Yerkes Laboratories in Orange Park, Florida [4, 6] until 1946, when he joined the University of Chicago as an assistant professor [4].

In 1949, he married Norma Gay Deupree; they had a son, Glenn Michael (Tad), and a daughter, Janeth Hope [4]. In 1952, Sperry was appointed as the Section Chief of Neurological Diseases and Blindness at the National Institutes of Health and then associate professor in Chicago. Although he planned to move to Bethesda, in 1954 he accepted an offer from the California Institute of Technology, where he became the inaugural Hixson Professor of Psychobiology.

Throughout his career, by adopting an experimental approach permeated by simplicity and lucidity [1], he challenged the ideas and theories of his doctoral and postdoctoral advisors, leading scientists in their fields [3]. At that time, it was widely believed that the developing nervous system was ‘a random, diffuse, unstructured, and essentially equipotential transmission network, a blank slate to be (…) shaped into a functionally adaptive communication system by use, experience, practice and learning’ [7]. Sperry performed surgical resections on fish, frogs, salamanders and rats to demonstrate, contrary to Paul Weiss’s theory, that nerves were guided to re-establish the same connections, whether it was the retina-brain connection or a foot innervation. Even interchanging the tendinous insertions of extensor and flexor muscles (and the nerves supplying them) did not lead to a re-learning [1]. With his chemoaffinity hypothesis, he echoed Cajal’s theoretical concepts of neurotropism and chemical selectivity [7]. These findings [8] make him a pioneer of the studies on axonal guidance. But it is not for these works that he won the Nobel Prize.

The corpus callosum, the largest commissural tract of the brain, connects the two hemispheres, but its functions were not known in the 1950s. Sperry and his PhD student Ronald Myers assessed visual deficits following the resection of the corpus callosum in cats [7]. This was the first chapter of a new research line on ‘split brains’, with studies also conducted in humans, to explore neurocognitive callosal functions in subjects surgically treated to limit the diffusion of epilepsy between the hemispheres [5]. Pioneering split-brain experiments, performed with Joseph Bogen and Michael Gazzaniga, showed that the right cerebral hemisphere has language abilities too and can perform visuospatial tasks better than the left one [1, 9, 10]. Other elegant works shed light on the duality of consciousness emerging after severing the corpus callosum, which stopped the information transfer between the two hemispheres; Sperry and co-workers opened up a new field on mind and consciousness at the crossroads between neuropsychology and philosophy, neuroscience and metaphysics [7]. He rejected Cartesian dualism, supported a mentalism theory and believed that consciousness was a ‘supervenient result of neural activity’ [7], emerging independently from the chemistry and biophysics of neurons [2].

Sperry won the Nobel Prize in 1981 for his breakthrough discoveries on functional specialisation of the hemispheres; the other half of the prize was jointly given to David Hubel and Torsten Wiesel for their studies on visual information processing. Among the many awards and honours received, Sperry was elected to the National Academy of Sciences in 1960, was elected Foreign Member of the Royal Society in 1976 and became a member of the Pontifical Academy of Sciences in 1978. In the following year, he was awarded the Albert Lasker Medical Research Award and the Wolf Prize in Medicine, and in 1989 he won the National Medal of Science.

After retiring, while suffering from a progressive neuromuscular degenerative disease, he continued working on consciousness, theories of mind and the mind-brain problem. He was described as a tenacious researcher [5] and a shy [2], taciturn person [6] who enjoyed boating, fishing and collecting fossils [4] and was in love with the beauty of remote places [3]. Sperry died after a heart attack on the 17th of April 1994 in Pasadena, California.

References

Hubel DH (1994) Roger W. Sperry (1913–1994). Nature 369(6477):186

CAS  Article  Google Scholar 

Todman D (2008) History of Neuroscience: Roger Sperry (1913–1994), IBRO History of Neuroscience http://www.ibro.info/Pub/Pub_Main_Display.asp?LC_Docs_ID=3473. Accessed on 1 Feb 2022

Voneida TJ (1997) Roger Wolcott Sperry. 20 August 1913-17 April 1994. Biogr Mem Fellows R Soc 43:463–470

Article  Google Scholar 

Sperry RW (1982) Biographical. Les Prix Nobel. The Nobel Prizes 1981. Stockholm, Editor Wilhelm Odelberg [Nobel Foundation]

Puente AE (1995) Roger Wolcott Sperry (1913–1994). Am Psychol 50:940–941

Article  Google Scholar 

Trevarthen C (1994) Roger W. Sperry (1913–1994). Trends Neurosci 17(10):402–404

CAS  Article  Google Scholar 

Berlucchi G (2006) Revisiting the 1981 Nobel Prize to Roger Sperry, David Hubel, and Torsten Wiesel on the occasion of the centennial of the Prize to Golgi and Cajal. J Hist Neurosci 15(4):369–375

Article  Google Scholar 

Sperry RW (1951) Mechanisms of neural maturation. In: Stevens S (ed) Handbook of experimental psychology. Wiley, New York

Google Scholar 

Gazzaniga MS, Bogen JE, Sperry RW (1965) Observations on visual perception after disconnection of the cerebral hemispheres in man. Brain 88:221–236

CAS  Article  Google Scholar 

Gazzaniga MS, Sperry RW (1967) Language after section of the cerebral commissures. Brain 99:131–148

Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author informationAuthors and AffiliationsCorresponding author

Correspondence to Stefano Sandrone.

Ethics declarations Conflicts of interest

No conflict of interest.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and Permissions

About this articleVerify currency and authenticity via CrossMarkCite this article

Sandrone, S. Roger W. Sperry (1913–1994). J Neurol 269, 5194–5195 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00415-022-11232-6

Download citation

Received: 14 February 2022

Revised: 14 June 2022

Accepted: 16 June 2022

Published: 22 July 2022

Issue Date: September 2022

DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00415-022-11232-6

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Get shareable link

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

留言 (0)

沒有登入
gif