Does Elevated Alcohol Consumption Delay the Diagnostic Assessment of Cognitive Impairment among Older Adults?

Introduction: The time from symptom debut to assessment of cognitive impairment (TSA) is usually substantial, and many factors can influence the length of this interval. Our objective was to discern whether elevated alcohol consumption is associated with TSA. Methods: Alcohol consumption was measured among 3,236 older Norwegians assessed for cognitive impairment. Elevated consumption was defined as drinking 4–7 times a week. TSA was defined as the number of months between symptom debut and assessment. The association between alcohol consumption and TSA was examined with a multiple regression analysis controlled for sociodemographic and clinical covariates. Results: Mean (SD) and median TSA were 34.8 (35.8) and 24.0 months, respectively. Elevated alcohol consumption was not associated with TSA. Longer TSA was associated with being male, having a high education level, being retired or unemployed, being single, having low scores on the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) or Personal Activities of Daily Living (PADL), having high subsyndrome scores of depression or agitation on The Neuropsychiatric Inventory – Questionnaire (NPI-Q), or having a spouse/cohabitant as the designated next of kin. Conclusion: This study indicates that elevated alcohol consumption does not influence TSA. Possible explanations are discussed, but further research is needed to determine the effect of alcohol definitively. We did identify other novel characteristics associated with TSA which may be important in minimizing the risk of delayed cognitive assessments and should be kept in mind when considering assessment.

© 2022 The Author(s). Published by S. Karger AG, Basel

Introduction

Dementia affects approximately 50 million people worldwide, and this number will quickly rise as the population of older adults will grow more rapidly in the coming decades [1]. It has an insidious onset and is characterized by cognitive, functional, and psychological impairment. It causes significant suffering to afflicted individuals and imposes an enormous economic cost on society at large [2].

Timely assessment of cognitive symptoms is important for many reasons, not least ruling out other treatable causes of cognitive impairment. Both neuropsychiatric and cognitive symptoms of dementia may be misinterpreted as other mental disorders [3, 4] or signs of normal aging [5-8]. If dementia is the underlying cause of cognitive impairment, however, timely assessment is crucial. More specifically, a timely assessment can make it easier to plan for the future [9-11], rule out preventable and reversible causes of symptoms [12-14], improve quality of life by treating cognitive and neuropsychiatric symptoms [15-17], ensure maximum effect of medical treatment [18-20], reduce the burden of care and delay nursing-home admissions [16, 21, 22], and increase cost-effectiveness [16, 22, 23].

Despite the importance of a timely assessment of cognitive symptoms, more than 50% of people with dementia may be undiagnosed [16, 24]. Accordingly, documenting the amount of time either between symptom debut and assessment (TSA) or between symptom debut and diagnosis has been important. Point estimates of TSA are typically between 1 and 2 years [25-28], although the variability is large, and some people wait much longer. Demographic variables such as sex, age, and education seem to be associated with the time until assessment or diagnosis, but the evidence is mixed [29-34]. In addition, young-onset dementia typically takes longer to diagnose than late-onset dementia [33, 35], and frontotemporal dementia takes longer than Alzheimer’s disease or vascular dementia [3, 33]. There are also various personal barriers preventing people from seeking assessment, such as denial, stigma, or unawareness of symptoms [36].

To our knowledge, no studies have examined whether elevated alcohol consumption influences TSA, nor the time until assessment or diagnosis of other health concerns. The focus of current research has been on how elevated alcohol consumption, across all ages, can delay or preclude assessment or treatment for alcohol use [37-40]. Importantly, however, this may also hold true for older adults with cognitive impairment [41]. One reason may be that general practitioners (GP) report being less willing to refer a person with cognitive symptoms to specialist care if this person also has an alcohol use disorder [41]. A possible consequence of this is that older adults with elevated alcohol consumption and cognitive impairment are less likely to receive a timely assessment of either problem. Furthermore, people with elevated alcohol consumption are less likely to have a stable informal network [42-46] and are at greater risk of developing a variety of neuropsychological impairments [47-51], some of which might lead to misattribution or reduced awareness of dementia symptoms. Importantly, the lack of network support, misattribution of dementia symptoms, and unawareness of dementia symptoms have all consistently been identified as barriers to receiving an assessment of cognitive impairment [36]. Taken together, these findings suggest that elevated alcohol consumption could affect TSA. If so, documenting such an effect would be important in ensuring that these older adults are not neglected and that they receive adequate medical attention. Thus, this study examined whether elevated alcohol consumption has an effect on TSA among older adults with symptoms of cognitive impairment.

Materials and MethodsParticipants

This study included older adults (≥60 years) registered in the Norwegian Registry of Persons Assessed for Cognitive Symptoms (NorCog) between 2014 and 2018. NorCog aims to standardize the assessment of cognitive impairment at specialist, outpatient clinics in Norway. The selection process is illustrated in Figure 1. A total of 2,683 cases were excluded due to ambiguous, incompatible, or missing (alcohol or TSA) data. Our final sample consisted of 3,236 cases.

Fig. 1.

Flowchart of the selection process. TSA, time from symptom debut to clinical assessment. *Two cases were excluded due to difficulties quantifying TSA accurately, and 1 case was excluded due to data suggesting the unlikely scenario that the case developed cognitive symptoms before the age of 20 years.

/WebMaterial/ShowPic/1403970Measures

NorCog provides a comprehensive, standardized test battery including physical, psychological, and cognitive examinations, as well as standardized interviews with both the patient and a next of kin. When all necessary examinations have been completed, a medical professional forms a diagnostic conclusion with regard to cognitive function. Table 1 outlines and describes the assessment scales used in our analyses, while Table 2 lists the entire selection of variables used. The outcome variable was the number of months between cognitive symptom debut and assessment (TSA), while the primary independent variable was alcohol consumption.

Table 1.

NorCog measurements included in study analyses

/WebMaterial/ShowPic/1403978Table 2.

Demographic and clinical characteristics of the study sample (n = 3,236)

/WebMaterial/ShowPic/1403976Statistical Analysis

The data were analyzed with SPSS version 26, STATA version 16, and Excel. A multiple linear regression analysis examined the relationship between variables of interest and the outcome variable, TSA, which was ln-transformed due to a skewed distribution. The relative effect of each variable in the multiple regression model was assessed by a dominance analysis. Cluster effect at the level of health institution was assessed with the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC). As ICC was close to zero, no adjustment was necessary. Tukey’s method identified 178 outliers at the level of TSA. These outliers were not omitted as the 5% trimmed mean was comparable to the overall sample mean, and because further investigation revealed that these cases reflect true numbers observed in Norwegian clinical practice; as such, they provide valuable information about the study population.

Missing values of the PADL and NPI-Q measures were imputed for cases with <50% missing item values. For each item, a random number drawn from an empirical distribution was used to replace missing values. Missing values of marital status were logically imputed. Because another 947 cases were excluded from the regression due to missing values, a weighted linear regression weighting cases more demographically similar to the excluded cases was performed post hoc. Ideally, dementia diagnosis would also have been included as a covariate in the regression analysis to account for diagnostic subtypes (see Table 2). However, it had a high amount of missing values, and there was no appropriate way to conduct imputation. Therefore, dementia diagnosis was excluded from further analysis, and MMSE scores were used to measure cognitive impairment.

Results

A total of 3,236 participants (52.3% female) with a mean (SD) age of 75.8 (7.2) years were included (Table 2). Approximately 56.1% were registered with a dementia diagnosis, 31.9% were registered with mild cognitive impairment (MCI), while the rest (12%) were registered with subjective cognitive impairment (SCI) or “others.” Mean (SD) MMSE score among the sample was 22.9 (4.6). Mean (SD) and median TSA were 34.8 (35.8) months and 24.0 months, respectively (min–max = 0–576 months). Figure 2 displays the cumulative distribution of TSA. A comparison between included cases (n = 3,236) and excluded cases (n = 1,435) (Table 3) revealed that included cases were slightly older, had fewer years of education, received domiciliary care less often, had lower mean MMSE scores, and had higher mean scores on the depression subsyndrome of the NPI-Q.

Table 3.

Test of differences between included and excluded cases (n = 4,671)

/WebMaterial/ShowPic/1403974Fig. 2.

Cumulative distribution of time from symptom debut to assessment (TSA).

/WebMaterial/ShowPic/1403968

The results of the regression analysis are presented in Table 4. Elevated alcohol consumption was not associated with TSA. Longer TSA was associated with being male, having more years of education, being retired or not working, and being single. Scoring higher on the depression and agitation subsyndrome scales of the NPI-Q was also associated with longer TSA. On the other hand, working ≥10%, having a more distant designated next of kin such as a neighbor or friend (compared to a partner), and scoring higher on the MMSE or PADL were associated with shorter TSA. The results of the weighted regression analysis (not reported) were equivalent to the results of the primary regression analysis.

Table 4.

Results of regression analysis of covariates related to TSA (n = 2,289)

/WebMaterial/ShowPic/1403972Discussion

This study aimed to discern whether elevated alcohol consumption has an effect on TSA among older Norwegians seeking assessment of cognitive impairment. Mean and median TSA were 34.8 and 24.0 months, respectively, comparable to what is typically reported. There was considerable variability, a consistent tendency in other studies, so it is evident that some people wait for many years before getting an assessment or receiving a correct diagnosis of cognitive impairment. This is disconcerting given that a delay of even a few years can be detrimental [11, 16, 64], so any factor that can reduce the wait is worth investigating. Elevated alcohol consumption was not associated with TSA. Longer TSA was associated with being male, having a high education level, being retired or not currently employed, being single, having low scores on the MMSE and PADL, having high NPI-Q subsyndrome scores of depression or agitation, or having a partner as the designated next of kin.

Unexpectedly, the results indicate that elevated alcohol consumption does not affect TSA. One reason for this may be the increased attention alcohol consumption among older adults has received in recent years. In the past, alcohol consumption among this group was frequently overlooked [65-67]. Recently, however, awareness of the importance of screening for alcohol consumption has been rising [68, 69]. This might translate to an increased willingness to refer older patients with cognitive symptoms to specialist assessment, to the point where elevated alcohol consumption may no longer cause significant delays in assessment. Alternatively, certain limitations of our study design might have made it challenging to uncover any effect of alcohol on TSA. For instance, there could be heterogeneity in alcohol consumption even among participants picking the same responses, as two participants drinking at the same frequency may nevertheless drink unequal amounts. We also excluded participants without a next of kin, who may drink more than people with an established next of kin. In addition, as explained in Table 1, our participants were initially examined by their GP, and NorCog does not document this visit. Thus, we cannot rule out the possibility that elevated alcohol consumption does indeed affect the length of time between symptom debut and the initial assessment performed by the GP, and that this effect is attenuated when the specialist health care assessment is used as the end point. Taken together, therefore, it is premature to conclude that elevated alcohol consumption does not affect TSA.

Surprisingly, having a closer designated next of kin resulted in longer TSA. Close next of kin are often the first to notice symptoms [25], which is likely one reason they are often so involved in help-seeking [70-73]. However, they do not always register the subtler changes related to cognitive impairment or may even deny their existence [5, 27, 74]. Two other demographic variables, marital and employment status, also yielded some novel results. Yet, it is unsurprising that being single could increase TSA because, as mentioned, close acquaintances are often instrumental in the help-seeking process. Therefore, it makes sense that our single participants, 92.0% of whom lived alone, had longer TSA. Furthermore, while low economic status could be a barrier to accessing health services [75], being employed could reduce TSA by virtue of taking place in a social arena where other people may notice cognitive symptoms.

The clinical measures also deserve mention. While higher scores of the depression subsyndrome of the NPI-Q could conceivably prolong TSA directly, this association could instead be due to cognitive impairment sometimes being misdiagnosed as depression, thereby delaying assessment or diagnosis of cognitive impairment [3, 33]. The agitation subsyndrome was also associated with longer TSA, replicating previous research [32]. Agitation can negatively affect the relationship between the person with cognitive impairment and their next of kin [76-81], and it is not unlikely that a strained relationship could interfere with help-seeking. Conversely, longer TSA could contribute to more severe neuropsychiatric symptoms, as these sometimes intensify as cognitive impairment progresses [82]. Furthermore, counterintuitive as it may seem, we are not the first to find that MMSE and PADL scores are inversely associated with time until assessment or diagnosis [29, 32]. One possible explanation is that people with mild cognitive symptoms have more insight than those with more severe symptoms and are more likely to recognize such symptoms and seek help [32, 83]. If so, given the intertwined relationship between daily function and cognition [84], this might explain why higher scores of both MMSE and PADL were associated with shorter TSA.

Strengths and Limitations

One limitation of our study is the use of retrospective reports about symptom debut, which are prone to memory errors. That said, all reports about symptom debut were systematically collected at the first specialist visit, reducing the risk of such errors to some degree. Another is the cross-sectional study design, which precludes conclusive causal explanations. Furthermore, 2,683 cases were excluded due to missing values, which introduces the risk of selection bias. However, between-group analyses (Table 3) found only minor differences between included and excluded cases, indicating minimal loss of representativity due to exclusion. An additional consequence of these exclusions, however, was that the regression analysis did not delineate between cases of SCI, MCI, and dementia. While MMSE scores should give an appropriate indication of cognitive impairment [85, 86], they do not adequately account for the heterogeneity in cognitive diagnoses. Also, as explained in the discussion, the null result regarding elevated alcohol consumption and TSA may be due to design limitations. Finally, there were outliers with high TSA that indicated that some participants waited several decades after symptom debut before being assessed. As mentioned previously, however, such numbers do occur in Norwegian clinical practice (e.g., due to misdiagnosis), and the inclusion of these outliers did not seem to affect our results considerably. This study also has notable strengths. Our large sample comprised individuals assessed at many different institutions from all health regions in Norway, and all assessments were standardized and consisted of internationally recognized tools.

Conclusion

This study found that elevated alcohol consumption was not associated with TSA, although more research is needed before conclusions can be drawn. Several other characteristics were associated with longer TSA, namely: being male, single, retired, or unemployed, having a high education level, low scores on the MMSE and PADL, high scores of depression or agitation, or having a partner as the designated next of kin. The presence of any one of these characteristics can increase the risk of undue delays of important assessments and should be kept in mind when deciding whether to pursue an assessment of cognitive impairment.

Statement of Ethics

Collection and storage of NorCog data were approved by the Norwegian Data Inspectorate. All NorCog participants signed an informed consent form that grants future use of their data. The present study was approved by the South-Eastern Regional Committee for Medical Research Ethics (REK sør-øst B: 21490) and the NorCog publication board.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

Funding Sources

This work was supported by the Innlandet Hospital Trust under Grant No. 150919.

Author Contributions

Anne-Sofie Helvik, Kjerstin Tevik, and Sverre Bergh conceptualized the study. Ben Kamsvaag contributed core design ideas, wrote the manuscript, performed data analysis, and performed literature searches. Kjerstin Tevik also performed literature searches. Jūratė Šaltytė Benth took lead in data analysis and performed the majority of the analyses. All authors, including Bei Wu and Geir Selbaek, were involved with determining which variables to include for statistical analysis, proofreading, and critical revision.

Data Availability Statement

The data in this study are not publicly available but may be requested from the Norwegian National Advisory Unit on Ageing and Health.

This article is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License (CC BY-NC). Usage and distribution for commercial purposes requires written permission. Drug Dosage: The authors and the publisher have exerted every effort to ensure that drug selection and dosage set forth in this text are in accord with current recommendations and practice at the time of publication. However, in view of ongoing research, changes in government regulations, and the constant flow of information relating to drug therapy and drug reactions, the reader is urged to check the package insert for each drug for any changes in indications and dosage and for added warnings and precautions. This is particularly important when the recommended agent is a new and/or infrequently employed drug. Disclaimer: The statements, opinions and data contained in this publication are solely those of the individual authors and contributors and not of the publishers and the editor(s). The appearance of advertisements or/and product references in the publication is not a warranty, endorsement, or approval of the products or services advertised or of their effectiveness, quality or safety. The publisher and the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to persons or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content or advertisements.

留言 (0)

沒有登入
gif