The surveys include “exit interviews,” questionnaires completed by proxy respondents after a participant's death, identified from the social network of the deceased. The exit interview provides detailed information on the respondent's final year of life and death circumstances.
The first cohort of HRS participants was interviewed in 1992; since then 5 additional cohorts have been included in the panel study in order to replenish the study sample. Our data include any age-eligible individual interviewed at least once. The target population for study sample initially included all US residents aged 51 - 61 who live in households, later expanded (by 1998) to include the entire population aged 51 and older. Following conventional practice for population surveys, institutionalized individuals (prisons, jails, nursing homes, long-term or dependent care facilities) are excluded from the initial survey population, although they are retained if in subsequent interviews they have moved to a nursing home. Baseline interviews are conducted face-to-face, while follow-up interviews are mostly conducted via telephone. Since 2006, at each wave, half of the respondents complete the face-to-face while the other half complete the core interview by telephone. The half-samples alternate waves so there is an in-person interview for each respondent every four years.
AnalysisThe presence of a living will is our outcome measure and it is used as an indicator of advance care planning. Beginning in 2002 a question regarding whether the deceased had written end-of-life instructions was included in the exit interview. Because the timing of end-of-life instructions was included in the questionnaire, we were able to reconstruct the presence of these for deceased participants prior to their death. For deceased participants without written end-of-life instructions we assumed that these were also not available at the times of previous interviews. Further, from 2012, the core HRS questionnaire included a question regarding the presence of the living will. If a living participant gave a negative response, then we assumed that the participant did not have a living will in all previous waves. In contrast, if a living participant confirmed the existence of a living will, we coded data in all previous waves as missing, as we were not sure when the end-of-life instructions were written. About 33% of deceased participants and 20% of living participants had a living will.
Ageing was represented using both: chronological age and remaining lifetime, with the latter serving as proxy for biological age. As people age, they are more aware that the end of their life is approaching. The aging process can be characterized through chronological age, which refers to amount of time the person has been alive, or through biological age, which refers to how old the person seems and which is related to genetic, behavioural, and environmental conditions[16Differences between biological and chronological age-at-death in human skeletal remains: A change of perspective.]. Biological age can be characterized using various biomarkers of ageing, but these are not routinely measured in population-level studies. On the other hand, if individuals are followed longitudinally, some will die during a follow up period. Remaining lifetime, or time to death (TTD) can be used as a proxy for biological age[17Wolf DA Freedman VA Ondrich JI Seplaki CL Spillman BC. Disability Trajectories at the End of Life: A "Countdown" Model.]. Research has shown that individuals’ expectations regarding their own future survival agree with actual experience, demonstrating that biomarkers were predictive of TTD regardless of age.[18Butler RN Sprott R Warner H et al.Biomarkers of aging: from primitive organisms to humans., 19Goldman N Turra CM Glei DA Seplaki CL Lin YH Weinstein M. Predicting mortality from clinical and nonclinical biomarkers., 20Modeling the rate of senescence: can estimated biological age predict mortality more accurately than chronological age?., 21Perceptions of Mortality: Individual Assessments of Longevity Risk.] The idea of using TTD in describing patients trajectories first appeared in 1970s and focused on hospital care of terminally ill patients[]. Later, few other studies considered TTD in assessing disability and classifying dying patients[17Wolf DA Freedman VA Ondrich JI Seplaki CL Spillman BC. Disability Trajectories at the End of Life: A "Countdown" Model.,23Lunney JR Lynn J Foley DJ Lipson S Guralnik JM. Patterns of functional decline at the end of life.]. Despite that, most studies take into account only chronological age, while ignoring the impact of TTD on late life events.A problem with TTD in panel studies is that for living participants it remains unknown, because only in rare occasions the sample will be followed up until all individuals die, removing the right-censoring problem[17Wolf DA Freedman VA Ondrich JI Seplaki CL Spillman BC. Disability Trajectories at the End of Life: A "Countdown" Model.]. Still, it can be estimated. Information on participants’ month and year of birth, death (if applicable), and month and year of interviews was used to establish measures of age and TTD at each interview. All age variables were expressed relative to age 75. For those who died during the follow up period, TTD is known and can never exceed 22 years. Participants that remain alive at the end of the follow up period have an unobserved value of TTD at each interview. We used interval regression to model remaining lifetime and to provide a basis for imputing the unobserved (censored) values of TTD. For cases with unknown an value of TTD, we constructed variables for lower and upper bounds of TTD. For cases with censored TTD, the lower bound of TTD is always known. For example, for someone still alive in 2014, we know that TTD is greater than 22 in 1992, and TTD is greater than 20 in 1994, and so on. So, at each interview the lower bound of TTD presents a difference between the age at final interview and the age at the current interview. We also assumed that the maximum possible age of participant is 112 years, as this was the age of the oldest deceased participant in the sample. The upper bound of TTD at each wave is defined as the difference between the maximum possible age and the current age of participant at each interview. Our interval regression for remaining lifetime imposes these bounds; when TTD is known (among uncensored cases) the upper and lower bounds are identical. Explanatory variables used in the regression include age, gender, indicator for racial and ethnicity status (Non-Hispanic White, Non-Hispanic Black, Hispanic White and Other), education level (below high-school level, high-school level, degree level), total assets, marital status (married or partnered, not-married or not-partnered), smoking and drinking status, self-reported health, census region (Midwest, Northwest, South, East and Other), and indicators for the following medical conditions: high blood pressure, diabetes, cancer, lung disease, heart disease, stroke, arthritis and psychiatric problems. These conditions are the most common comorbidities among the elderly [24Diederichs C Berger K Bartels DB. The measurement of multiple chronic diseases–a systematic review on existing multimorbidity indices.]. The estimated regression was used to impute TTD to the censored observations, with the known lower- and upper-bound conditions imposed. Because we are using a linear model of remaining lifetime, it produces an unbiased estimate of the expected value of remaining lifetime among those for whom TTD is not observed.We used random-effects logistic regression to estimate the relationship between the probability of having a living will and a set of time-varying and time-invariant control variables. To account for the uncertainty present in the TTD imputation process, we performed 5 independent sets of imputations, and used multiple imputation estimation and inference techniques in the analysis. Explanatory variables included age and TTD in linear and quadratic form, gender, indicator for racial and ethnicity status, education level, marital status, income and assets, census region, and indicators for excellent self-reported health (health status is very good or excellent), and the presence of the eight medical conditions listed above. All analyses were performed using the statistical software STATA (Version 14).
ResultsOn average, individuals were 66 years old over the 12 survey waves (Table 1). Average estimated TTD for the analysed sample was 16.3 years, while average observed TTD for individuals with observed deaths was 7.2 years. Majorities of the sample were non-Hispanic Whites (72.8%), female (60.3%) and had a high-school degree or more (73.2%). Arthritis was the most common chronic condition reported by 49.3% of participants, while stroke was the least common condition, reported by only 5.7% of the sample. Table 1.Table 1Sample characteristics
Notes: TTD denotes “time-to-death”. ADL denotes “activities of daily living”.
A range of individual characteristics affected the presence of the living will (Table 2). Age [OR (Age)=1.85, 95%CI (1.81 – 1.90), pTable 2Table 2Random-effects logistic regression analysis of determinants of planning at the end-of-life
Notes: Presented results are from random effects logistic regression analysis. Results are presented as odds ratios. Odds ratio indicates percentage odds change for a unit increase in the observed variable, holding other variables constant.
As individual end-of-life planning trajectories depend both on age and TTD, there are numerous pathways in progress within the general population at any moment. We illustrate selected scenarios in Figure 1 and show how they are modified by various background factors in Figure 2, Figure 3. In all cases, the fitted probabilities reflect the mix of sample characteristics in all other respects. Figure 1 illustrates the pattern of having a living will for individuals who die at age 80, 90 and 100, depending on current age. Individuals who die at a very old age (e.g. 100 years) will almost certainly have a living will immediately prior to death (97.5%), while the analogous probability for individuals who die at age 80 is much lower (38.1%). Lastly, most individuals don't initiate a living will until age 75 or older. Figure 1Figure 1Probability of having a living will, by current age and age at death. Notes: The graph depicts an average sample population for different ages at death.
Figure 2Probability of having a living will, by current age and age at death for individuals of different ethnical and racial background. Notes: The graph depicts an average sample population of different ethnical and racial background for different ages at death. Effect of Non-Latino Black is similar to the effect of Latino White and therefore trajectories of Non-Latino Black are indicative of those of Latino White.
Figure 3Probability of having a living will, by current age and age at death for different educational levels. Notes: HS denotes “high-school”. The graph depicts an average sample population that has different levels of educational attainment for different ages at death.
Non-Hispanic Whites initiate end-of-life planning much earlier compared to those of other ethnic and racial backgrounds, and also end up with much higher levels or participation (Figure 2). For example, Non-Hispanic Whites dying at age 100 will almost certainly have a living will (99.2%), while the same probability among Non-Hispanic Blacks dying at the same age is much lower (37.5%). The differences are even larger for individuals who die younger, indicating the importance of ethnicity and race for end-of-life planning. Figure 2More educated individuals are more likely to participate in end-of-life planning and, on average, do so earlier compared to their less-educated counterparts (Figure 3). The differences are particularly high for individuals who die younger. For example, a college-educated individual who dies at age 80 years has 79.1% probability of having a living will, while someone with only a basic education dying at the same age has only a 2.4% probability of having a living will. Figure 3DiscussionThe study provides insights into factors that affect advance care planning. Our results, based on a nationally representative sample of Americans, provide key insights into factors that indicate variation in how and when Americans perform advance care planning. We show that despite concerted efforts to push advance care planning upstream, few individuals undertake advance care planning before the age of 75, and only about a third of patients who die at age 80 adopt an advance care plan. Furthermore, while patients with cancer are most likely to have an advance care plan, patients with heart disease, the most common cause of death in the US, are least likely to have an advance care plan.
Our results provide unique insights into how biological and chronological age are associated with advance care planning. If TTD is viewed as a proxy for biological age, our results indicate that it is chronological age, and not biological age, that exerts a stronger influence on the likelihood of having a living will[17Wolf DA Freedman VA Ondrich JI Seplaki CL Spillman BC. Disability Trajectories at the End of Life: A "Countdown" Model.]. Further, worsened health status is also positively correlated with participation in end-of-life planning. However, even though aging and health status matter, an individual's socio-economic and racial/ethnic circumstances appear to have a strong association with the propensity to plan for the end-of-life. Non-Hispanic Whites and more educated Americans have considerably higher rates of end-of-life planning participation.Serious illnesses significantly increase someone's likelihood of having a living will though not all diseases have a similar impact. A cancer diagnosis is often presented to a patient as a “death sentence,” and may be accompanied by a prognosis regarding remaining lifetime likely prompting patents without living wills to consider adopting one. Additionally, as cancer patients tend to have better access to palliative services, they may also have a better awareness of end-of-life planning options and their significance[25Barriers to access to palliative care.]. However, patients with heart disease, the most common cause of death in the US, are least likely to have an advance care plan, suggesting opportunities to improve care and communication for this large group of patients. Prior work has shown that both patients with heart disease and their physicians are likely to underestimate their risk of mortality compared to cancer, which could reduce their likelihood of documenting an advance care plan since it alters their perceived time to death[26Allen LA Yager JE Funk MJ et al.Discordance Between Patient-Predicted and Model-Predicted Life Expectancy Among Ambulatory Patients With Heart Failure.,27Warraich HJ Allen LA Mukamal KJ Ship A Kociol RD. Accuracy of physician prognosis in heart failure and lung cancer: comparison between physician estimates and model predicted survival.].Our results are consistent with recent studies on end-of-life planning[28McAfee CA Jordan TR Sheu JJ Dake JA Kopp Miller BA Predicting Racial and Ethnic Disparities in Advance Care Planning Using the Integrated Behavioral Model.,29Advance Care Planning in palliative care: a systematic literature review of the contextual factors influencing its uptake 2008-2012.]. Older individuals and those in poorer health are more aware that death is approaching and are more inclined to engage in care planning activities that could relieve pain and discomfort in their last moments of life[1Institute of Medicine (US)Noah BA, Feigenson N. Avoiding Overtreatment at the End of Life: Physician-Patient Communication and Truly Informed Consent. 2016.
].In the US, advance care planning has been widely promoted as a tool for communicating end-of-life preferences[40Yadav KN Gabler NB Cooney E et al.Approximately One In Three US Adults Completes Any Type Of Advance Directive For End-Of-Life Care.]. Despite these efforts, completion rates remain low. According to a recent systematic review, only about one third of Americans has completed some form of advance care planning[40Yadav KN Gabler NB Cooney E et al.Approximately One In Three US Adults Completes Any Type Of Advance Directive For End-Of-Life Care.]. Since 2016 Medicare reimburses physicians for having end-of-life conversations, and early evidence suggests that these conversations are associated with less intensive end-of-life care[42Gupta A Jin G Reich A et al.Association of Billed Advance Care Planning with End-of-Life Care Intensity for 2017 Medicare Decedents.]. Also, advance care planning includes several legal conditions, required for executing advance directives, such as qualified witnesses and notarization[40Yadav KN Gabler NB Cooney E et al.Approximately One In Three US Adults Completes Any Type Of Advance Directive For End-Of-Life Care.]. Even though these restrictions are put in place to protect the patients, these can also be barriers for those that might not fully understand the legal system and cannot afford legal counselling.This study has several limitations. The analysis utilises exit interviews which are conducted with a proxy respondent, which might lead to missing or erroneous information regarding the existence and content of living wills. Nevertheless, most proxy-respondents (88%) are close family members and they are likely aware of care planning activities of their loved ones. Further, family and health care professionals may have an important role in individual's end-of-life decision-making, but unfortunately the used data does not allow investigations of their impact to end-of-life decisions and living will content. Also, it was not possible to assess the patient's quality of end-of-life or family satisfaction with the patient death quality and to investigate the relationship between having a living will and the end-of-life care model (e.g. ICU or palliative care). Another potential weakness is the imputation of TTD. While we used an extensive set of possible predictors, we cannot exclude a possibility of unobserved heterogeneity that could bias our estimates. Finally, even though our random-effects logistic model controls for a wide range of individual characteristics that may affect advance care planning, some important determinants might remain unobserved. Therefore, our findings should be interpreted as associations and not as causal effects.
Advance care planning is an important step for the provision of patient-centred and cost-effective care[43
留言 (0)