1Temple University (Emeritus), Philadelphia, PA, USA; 2University of Arizona (Adjunct), Tucson, AZ, USA; 3Visual and Autonomous Exploration Systems Research Laboratory, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, USA
We propose that pain should be included as an integral part of fight-or-flight response in publications, presentations, teaching materials, and clinical practice.
In 1915, W.B. Cannon described two “instincts” that occur in animals in the context of “emotional excitement”.1 That work is best remembered for establishing the concept, now known as flight or fight response (F/F). In essence, F/F consists of automatically activated defensive behaviors to deal with threats.2 Subsequent work, now known as “predatory imminence continuum theory”,3 identifies and differentiates defensive behaviors in F/F into three sequential aspects or states: pre-encounter (recognized threat, but at a distance), post-encounter (threat within closing distance), and circa-strike (imminent or actual attack). Each state relates to the progressive degree of threat and corresponds to progressive states of anxiety, fear, and panic.3 Specific brain regions are associated with each defensive state,4 and brain-region-“switching” to active defense (F/F) occurs as the threat progresses.5
Pain is not important as a survival message in the pre- or post-encounter states but would be in the circa-strike state, that is, the actual attack. The neuroanatomical connection between pain and F/F involves the descending pain-modulating pathways.6 Switching during progressive danger imminence, which requires active F/F behavior, has been shown to occur in the midbrain periaqueductal gray (PAG), a region involved in descending pain modulation.7–9 In mice, distinct coding characteristics in the PAG are manifested during flight vs “freezing” defensive responses to threat;10 in rats, studies using early gene (c-fos) expression revealed that the pattern of pain activation within the PAG “reflected a quality more akin to the ‘behavioral significance’ of the noxious event”, that is, an “escapability dimension” involving “the emotional strategy (active or passive) used by the animal to cope with the noxious event”.11 The shift in threat imminence also occurs in humans. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and an active avoidance paradigm, in which volunteers were virtually pursued through a maze by a predator endowed with the ability to chase, capture, and inflict pain, brain activity shifted from planning avoidance strategies in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex to the PAG as the predator grew closer.12
Our proposal to include pain as an integral part of the fight-or-flight response has been anticipated by many publications and presentations that start out by stating that pain is beneficial since it alerts to impending tissue damage, the recount of Dr. Livingstone’s attack by a lion, “he shook me as a terrier dog does a rat. The shock produced a stupor similar to that felt by a mouse after the first shake of a cat. It caused a sort of dreaminess, in which there was no sense of pain or feeling of terror, though quite conscious of all that was happening”,13 and even Freud’s conjecture that human psychological defense against internal threat evolved from animal defense against external predatory threat.14 However, to our knowledge, this is the first explicit proposal that pain is always included in descriptions of the physiological components of the fight-or-flight response.
DisclosureProfessor Robert Raffa reports personal fees from Neumentum, during the conduct of the study; personal fees from Enalare, outside the submitted work. The authors report no other conflicts of interest in this work.
References1. Cannon WB. Bodily Changes in Pain, Hunger, Fear and Rage: An Account of Recent Researches into the Function of Emotional Excitement. D. Appleton; 1915.
2. Kozlowska K, Walker P, McLean L, Carrive P. Fear and the defense cascade: clinical implications and management. Harv Rev Psychiatry. 2015;23(4):263–287. doi:10.1097/HRP.0000000000000065
3. Fanselow MS, Lester LS. A functional behavioristic approach to aversely motivated behavior: predatory imminence as a determinant of the topography of defensive behavior. In: Bolles RC, Beecher MD, editors. Evolution and Learning. Erlbaum; 1988:185–211.
4. Deng H, Xiao X, Wang Z. Periaqueductal gray neuronal activities underlie different aspects of defensive behaviors. J Neurosci. 2016;36(29):7580–7588. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4425-15.2016
5. Hashemi MM, Gladwin TE, de Valk NM, et al. Neural dynamics of shooting decisions and the switch from freeze to fight. Sci Rep. 2019;9(1):4240. doi:10.1038/s41598-019-40917-8
6. Millan MJ. Descending control of pain. Prog Neurobiol. 2002;66(6):355–474. doi:10.1016/s0301-0082(02)00009-6
7. Mayer DJ, Wolfle TL, Akil H, Carder B, Liebeskind JC. Analgesia from electrical stimulation in the brainstem of the rat. Science. 1971;174(4016):1351–1354. doi:10.1126/science.174.4016.1351
8. Reynolds DV. Surgery in the rat during electrical analgesia induced by focal brain stimulation. Science. 1969;164(3878):444–445. doi:10.1126/science.164.3878.444
9. Loyd DR, Murphy AZ. The role of the periaqueductal gray in the modulation of pain in males and females: are the anatomy and physiology really that different? Neural Plast. 2009;2009:462879. doi:10.1155/2009/462879
10. Liu D, Li S, Ren L, Liu X, Li X, Wang Z. Different coding characteristics between flight and freezing in dorsal periaqueductal gray of mice during exposure to innate threats. Animal Model Exp Med. 2022;5(6):491–501. doi:10.1002/ame2.12276
11. Keay KA, Clement CI, Depaulis A, Bandler R. Different representations of inescapable noxious stimuli in the periaqueductal gray and upper cervical spinal cord of freely moving rats. Neurosci Lett. 2001;313(1–2):17–20. doi:10.1016/s0304-3940(01)02226-1
12. Mobbs D, Petrovic P, Marchant JL, et al. When fear is near: threat imminence elicits prefrontal-periaqueductal gray shifts in humans. Science. 2007;317(5841):1079–1083. doi:10.1126/science.1144298
13. Anonymous. Livingstone’s fight with a lion: a cast of his fractured humerus received in Chicago. Med Examiner. 1874;15(21):527–529.
14. Schwartz PJ. Freud’s 1926 conjecture is confirmed: evidence from the dorsal periaqueductal gray in mice that human psychological defense against internal instinctual threat evolved from animal motor defense against external predatory threat. Front Psychol. 2024;15:1427816. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1427816
留言 (0)