Weight Misperception is Prevalent Among Turkish Adults and Associated with Higher Age, Body Mass Index and Lower Education Status

Introduction

Excess body weight is a global health issue that stems from an unhealthy diet, reduced exercise, and an imbalance between energy intake and expenditure.1 Patients with excess body weight are classified as overweight or obese. Body mass index (BMI) is the most commonly used anthropometric measurement to diagnose overweight and obesity. It is calculated as follows: Body mass is divided by the square of height and expressed as kg/m2. Adults with a BMI of 25.0–29.9 kg/m2 are classified as overweight, whereas adults with a BMI of 30 and over are classified as obese.1 Various cardiovascular, metabolic, renal, and oncologic disorder’s etiology and progression are tightly associated with excess body weight.2–8 These close associations have yielded the development of dietary, pharmacologic, device-based, and surgical therapies for excess body weight and obesity with weight loss rates of 10 to 25%, which are related to improved health indices.9,10

Weight perception is how one perceives their weight and their physical appearance.11 It has been shown that altered weight perception is common, especially among adolescents and young people, and is associated with psychiatric conditions such as depression, suicidality, and disordered eating.12–14 Studies about weight misperception illustrate that almost one in two young people perceives their weight inappropriately.15,16 Moreover, weight misperception rates are even higher (72%) among overweight and obese youth. Most importantly, weight misperception was demonstrated to be negatively correlated with weight loss trials among adolescents and youngsters.15,17

Adult populations are also susceptible to weight misperception, not just youngsters. Studies have demonstrated that more than 1 in 5 Korean female adults and up to 2 in 3 Saudi Arabian adults have weight misperception.18,19 Some of the factors associated with weight misperception were age, level of education, and marital status in the former study; and obesity, age-group, educational level, diagnosed chronic condition, self-rated health, and sedentary lifestyle in the latter. Similar to the studies on adolescents, it has been shown that weight misperception among young adults is also linked with psychiatric conditions such as disordered eating habits.20 Besides psychological issues, studies have demonstrated that weight misperception in adults is associated with lower self-rated health scores and a lower health-related quality of life.21,22

Through our clinical practice, we have observed that weight misperception may play an important yet unrecognized hampering role to the treatment of overweight or obesity among adults. Many patients with obesity, or patients who are overweight with related disorders, refuse obesity treatment because they believe that they are not overweight or obese. Similar to the weight misperception studies that were performed on Korean and Saudi Arabian adults,18,19 we have observed that weight misperception among adults may be related to social determinants of health (SDoH). It is known that SDoH is associated with obesity as well.23,24 We planned a two-step study on adult weight misperception. This study forms the first step, with the aim of describing the characteristics of weight misperception and its association with SDoH among the general population, namely among adults with or without excess body weight. Findings from this study will be used for designing our step two study, which is planned to be conducted on patients with excess body weight to evaluate their obesity treatment approaches from the perspective of body misperception. To the best of our knowledge, there is no study to evaluate weight misperception among adults in Turkey and its possible effect on obesity treatment utilization.

Methodology Design and Setting

The study was designed as a descriptive research survey and took place in two academic center’s general internal medicine outpatient clinics from August 1st, 2023, to November 1st, 2023, in Ankara, Turkey. Patients who were admitted to these two clinics for various reasons were informed about the survey at the end of their medical visit and were asked whether they would like to participate. If they opted to participate, written informed consent was obtained. The patients were given an anonymous survey number and proceeded with the questions. No identifying name or number was acquired. The whole survey took approximately ten to fifteen minutes to complete.

Survey

The survey was developed by the authors of the study and is not validated. Weight misperception was assessed both visually and verbally via the classification system that was mainly developed from modifications to previous questionnaires and methods.18,25 The survey consisted of twenty questions in three parts (Supplementary Material 1). To maintain consistency, the same researcher (BCE) asked the questions for each patient. All questions were read by the researcher, and each question was explained in detail to participants. The first part consisted of demographics, dietary habits, and social determinants of health. The second part was related to anthropometric measurements and comorbidities. The third and last part was created for the assessment of verbal and visual weight perceptions.

Assessment of Verbal Weight Perception

We used an assessment method similar to one that was used in a recent study.18 Patients were asked to describe themselves as one of the following: underweight, normal-weighted, overweight, mildly obese, or severely obese. This question was read twice to make sure patients comprehended it correctly.

Assessment of Visual Weight Perception

A previously developed and validated body size guide (BSG) was used for visual perception analysis.25 Patients were shown the images according to their sex and were asked to point out the image they found most similar to themselves.

Appropriate Perception

Appropriate perception (AP) was defined by the authors as “having a BMI in total concordance with both visual and verbal weight perception question’s answers”. For example, if a patient’s BMI was calculated as 31.2 kg/m2 (mildly obese), they described themselves verbally as “mildly obese”, and they pointed out “mildly obese” images in their visual BSG, then this patient was labeled as having “appropriate perception”.

Misperceptions and Classifications

Misperception classification was not straightforward, and several misperception classifications were needed due to the following reasons: Firstly, misperception can be the perception of being “thinner” or “fatter” than reality. Secondly, misperception can be “visually”, “verbally”, or “both”. Finally, one can perceive themselves as thinner on a visual scale, or fatter verbally, or vice versa. The term “misperception” was used as an umbrella term to encompass both verbal and visual misperceptions of being thinner than reality or fatter than reality. Three main misperception groups created were thinner than reality misperception (TTRM), fatter than reality misperception (FTRM), and either thinner or fatter than reality misperception (ETFTRM), a group containing any kind of misperception. Misperception and its classification are provided in Supplementary Material 2 in detail.

Statistics

Categorical and continuous variables were first analyzed via descriptive statistical methods. Differences between misperception groups and categorical determinants were analyzed using Pearson’s chi-squared test (χ2 test) (or Fisher’s exact test when needed). Differences between misperception groups and continuous variables were analyzed using the Student’s t-test or Mann–Whitney U-test, according to the variable’s distribution patterns. Continuous variables were presented as “mean (± standard deviation)” or “median (interquartile range)” according to their distribution pattern. Categorical variables were presented as “numbers (percentages)”. Univariate logistic regression was performed to analyze the determinants of misperception. Variables with a p< 0.02 were tested in multivariate logistic regression analysis. Kappa statistics were used to determine the strength of agreement between weight perception and actual BMI. Two-sided significance testing was performed to calculate p-values, and p-values less than 0.05 were considered significant. All analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS Software version 23.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL).

Sample Size

We have conducted a literature search to find similar studies conducted where this study was planned. Although we could not find any related data regarding adults, we have identified a study in Turkey regarding adolescent’s weight perception, which illustrated 50% misperception.16 Since this data would not be ideal to use for our sampling, we used it for preliminary sample sizing and planned to conduct interim analysis to calculate the misperception rate of adults, which turned out to be much higher (to be discussed in the results section). With a 95% confidence interval and a 5% margin of error, at least 246 participants were calculated to be needed for the study.

Ethics

Each patient in the study was assigned an anonymous identification number to protect confidentiality. Written informed consent was obtained prior to survey initiation. The study complies with the principles outlined in the Declaration of Helsinki, and this study was approved by the Başkent University Institutional Review Board on August 1, 2023 (Project number KA23/275).

Results Demographics and Social Status

Two hundred and fifty patients participated in the study. Of whom, 178 were female (71.2%). 107 (42.8%) patient’s highest education level was primary school and the remainder were similar between middle or high school and college at 28.8% and 28.4%, respectively. The majority of the patients (78%) were married and the remainder were similar between single and divorced/widow, at 10.4% and 11.6%, respectively. Only 96 (38.4%) patients had a regular job, whereas 146 (58.4%) were not working and 8 (3.2%) had irregular jobs. 135 (54%) patients described their living conditions as moderate, and the rest were almost similar; 51 (20.4%) were described as easy, and 64 (25.6%) were described as difficult. Parts 1 and 2 of Table 1 demonstrate the detailed data regarding demographics and social statistics.

Table 1 Demographic, Social, Nutritional, Habitual, and Anthropometric Characteristics of the Patients

Nutritional Status and Access to Food

All patients except for two (0.8%) described themselves as omnivores. While access to balanced and healthy food was answered as moderate by 122 (48.8) patients, 79 (31.6%) answered as easy and 49 (19.6%) as difficult. Access to dairy products, bakery products, and fresh fruit and vegetables was answered as easy by the majority, with rates of 50.4%, 71.2%, and 62.4%, respectively. Access to meat and products, however, was moderate for 37.6% of the patients, easy for 32.4%, and difficult for 30%. Part 3 of Table 1 illustrates the detailed data regarding nutrition and access to food.

Habits, Anthropometric Measurement, and Morbidities

Only 70 (28%) patients were currently smoking; the remainder were either former smokers or non-smokers, and 77.2% of the patients were not consuming alcohol. The median height, weight, and BMI for females were 160 (8) cm, 73 (19) kg, and 28.2 (6.9) kg/m2, whereas they were 172 (11) cm, 83 (15) kg, and 26.9 kg/m2 for males, respectively. Hypertension and diabetes were the two most common morbidities among the patients. While 20.8% had no morbidity, 56.4% had either one or two morbidities, and 22.8% had three or more morbidities. Parts 4, 5, and 6 of Table 1 describe the detailed data regarding habits, anthropometric measurements, and morbidities.

General Weight Perceptions

Of the 250 patients, only 47 (18.8%) perceived themselves appropriately on both visual and verbal scales, whereas 98 (39.2%) had both visual and verbal misperceptions. The remainder have either verbal or visual misperceptions. Regarding visual perception, 42% perceived it appropriately, but the remainder had either thinner or fatter than reality misperceptions. With regards to verbal perception, 36.8% had appropriate perception, but the remainder, 63.2%, had either thinner or fatter than reality misperception. The agreement between the actual BMI and the visual weight perception was only slight; however, the agreement between the actual BMI and the verbal weight perceptions was very poor (κ = 0.253, SE = 0.039, P<0.001, and κ = 0.121, SE = 0.039, P=0.001, respectively). The strength of agreement between the verbal and visual weight perceptions was also slight (κ = 0.207, SE = 0.034, P<0.001). Part 1 of Table 2 illustrates detailed data regarding general misperceptions.

Table 2 Detailed Body Perception Analysis and Misperception Subgroups of the Patients

Misperception Groups and Subgroups

The most detailed misperception analysis in this study was acquired by creating subgroups and allocating them to the relevant misperception group. 113 (45.2%) patients had thinner than reality misperception (TTRM), and 56 (22.4%) patients had fatter than reality misperception (FTRM). 34 (13.6%) patients were in subgroup 5, which has opposing visual and verbal perceptions (ie, thinner than reality verbal perception but fatter than reality visual perception, or vice versa). Parts 2 and 3 of Table 2 illustrate detailed data regarding misperception groups.

Misperception Group’s Distribution Among Determinants

Age and BMI distributions were significantly different between misperception groups (Table 3). Patients in both ETFTRM and TTRM groups were significantly older (57 vs 47, p<0.001 and 57 vs 53, p =0.005). With respect to BMI, patients with ETFTRM and TTRM had significantly higher BMIs (28.5 vs 23.8, p<0.001 and 30.4 vs 26.8, p<0.001), yet patients with FTRM had lower BMI values (26.8 vs 28.4, p =0.003). Table 3 illustrates the misperception group’s distribution among age and BMI in detail.

Table 3 Age and BMI Values According to Different Groups of Misperceptions

Regarding categorical variables, the most remarkable distribution differences were observed in education and marital status. TTRM decreased as the level of education increased, and the TTRM was significantly higher among primary school-educated patients compared to middle- or high-school and college-educated patients. On the opposite side, FTRM paralleled education status. FTRM was significantly higher when patient’s highest education level was at least middle school. ETFFTRM, which denotes any kind of misperception, was significantly higher at the primary school level compared to patients who have a college degree. With regards to marital status, TTRM and ETFTRM were significantly low among single patients; however, FTRM did not differ according to marital status. TTRM, FTRM, and ETFTRM misperceptions were similar among different living conditions, access to balanced and healthy food, and access to various food groups. Table 4 illustrates misperception group’s distribution among categorical determinants in detail.

Table 4 Demographic characteristics of participating patients (n = 323)

Predictors of Misperception

Univariate logistic regression analysis was performed to identify predictors of misperception. Higher age, higher BMI, primary school being the highest education status, and not being single in marital status were associated with ETFTRM and TTRM. Regarding FTRM, predictors were opposite since lower BMI and higher education levels were associated with higher FTRM. Table 5 illustrates the details of the univariate logistic regression analysis. These variables were also tested in multivariate logistic regression to analyze independent predictors of ETFTRM, TTRM, and FTRM. The model for predicting TTRM illustrated that the model consisting of higher age, higher BMI, and lower education status was an independent predictor of TTRM. Table 6 demonstrates multivariate logistic regression analysis in detail. Multivariate logistic regression analysis for ETFTRM and FTRM did not yield satisfactory results, therefore they were not included in the study.

Table 5 Univariate Logistic Regression Analysis of Variables on Different Groups of Misperceptions

Table 6 Multivariate Logistic Regression Analysis of Variables on Different Groups of Misperceptions

Discussion

This study demonstrated that weight misperception is highly prevalent among Turkish adults, with similar rates seen in adults in Saudi Arabia, Malaysia, Nigeria, and the Netherlands,19,26–28 but considerably higher than rates seen in South Korea and Mauritius.29,30 Whether the differences in weight perception rates among countries are factual or stem from the fact that there is no universally validated assessment tool, each researcher uses their own method. Therefore, our findings cannot be generalized to the entire Turkish adult population.

Due to the lack of a universal assessment tool and the dependence on individual researchers, each researcher employed a unique technique. Joo et al’s18 technique has similarities with our study’s verbal assessment part; however, we differ from their study due to the fact that we did not only use verbal assessment but also used Harris et al’s25 visual scale. Although both verbal and visual scales’ agreement strengths with actual BMI were low, since the visual scale had better kappa statistics than the verbal scale, further studies that will use the visual scale to assess weight perception may end up having lower weight misperception ratios compared to ones that were acquired via verbal scales.

Our study demonstrated that TTRM, namely, weight underestimation, was associated with aging, a higher BMI, and a lower education level, the outcomes that were also reported in recent studies.18,19,28,30 Althumiri et al demonstrated that misperception occurs more frequently in those who do not have a bachelor’s degree.19 Our study demonstrated that there is no single threshold and misperception rates gradually decrease as the level of education increases. This gradual decline was also shown in various studies.27,29,30 Gender’s role in weight perception is controversial. While several studies indicate that gender may be associated with weight misperception,20,29,30 our study, along with the others,19,28 did not find any association. Different cultural backgrounds strongly influence women’s body image, which could explain this difference. For example, overweight women may be regarded as more beautiful in some Middle Eastern and Sub-Saharan African countries, in contrast to western and Chinese culture.30–33 Hence, the presence or absence of gender’s role may not be generalizable to every other country. Studies consistently show a correlation between age and increased weight misperception, with the exception of a Nigerian study, which found a link between weight misperception and young adult age.27 Considering the fact that women are more susceptible than men to societally imposed ideal beauty standards,34 and this susceptibility weakens with aging,33 differences in the study characteristics may be partially responsible for the contrasting findings in the studies.

Another factor for which our study found no association, but other studies have suggested a relationship, is marital status. Marital status was a determinant of WM in univariate logistic regression, with being either married or divorced/widowed being associated with weight misperception compared to being single, but there was no difference between them. However, in multivariate logistic regression, they were not found to have an independent association with weight misperception. A recent Malaysian study indicated that being divorced was significantly associated with WM compared to being married (odds ratio: 4.70, CI: 1.44–15.32, P = 0.01).26 Similarly, a Korean study also indicated an association between marital status and WM.29 Most of the participants (78%) in our study were married, and only 55 participants were either single or divorced/widowed. The low number of participants in our study is likely to impede a possible association between marital status and WM.

Excess body weight and obesity pathogenesis are not fully understood, yet various genetic and environmental factors have been demonstrated to contribute to this altered energy metabolism syndrome. It has been shown that sex, age, and socioeconomic status have an impact on weight gain. Excess body weight was more likely among women, the elderly, and those of low socioeconomic status. Moreover, lower education is also associated with a higher prevalence of obesity.35,36 It is intriguing that, similar to the factors associated with obesity, these risk factors were also shown to be associated with higher odds of weight misperception in adults.19,26,28–30 We cannot draw a conclusion from this study that weight misperception is the underlying cause of obesity among the aforementioned groups; however, this hypothesis deserves future research.

Dietary therapy is the cornerstone of overweight or obese treatment strategies. While they are effective in the short term, their long-term efficacy is usually disappointing due to diminished dietary adherence. We do not fully understand the mechanisms of dietary adherence, but we know they are associated with sociocultural/environmental, cognitive, ingestive, and absorptive variables.37 Researchers have never studied or defined weight misperception as a predictor of dietary adherence. Our research shows that there is a positive relationship between weight misperception and body mass index. This relationship can also help people with TTRM stay on track with their diets.

Our study revealed that while appropriate perception is similar between the visual scale and verbal question (42% vs 36.8%), verbal weight misperception was more prevalent when it came to the perception of being thinner than reality, and visual weight misperception was more prevalent when it came to the perception of being fatter than reality. This finding suggests that we should not use the visual scale and verbal question for perception analysis interchangeably, as the former is more effective in identifying patients who perceive themselves as fatter than reality, while the latter is more effective in identifying patients who perceive themselves as thinner than reality.

We have revealed that t-SG 2, namely visual misperception but verbal accurate perception among the thinner than reality misperception group, was significantly lower than t-SG 3, namely verbal misperception but visual accurate perception (2.4% vs 21.2%). Contrary to this finding, f-SG 2, namely visual misperception but verbal accurate perception among the fatter than reality misperception group, was significantly higher than f-SG 3, namely verbal misperception but visual accurate perception (15.6% vs 2%). We interpreted this finding as saying that those who misperceive themselves as thinner than reality tend to articulate their perception inappropriately but visualize themselves in their minds appropriately, while those who misperceive themselves as fatter than reality tend to articulate their perception appropriately but visualize themselves in their minds inappropriately. This finding may be attributed to different weight misperception mechanisms between TTRM and FTRM.

We acknowledge the limitations of our study. Firstly, we conducted this study in two academic centers, which may limit the generalizability of the findings to the entire Turkish population. Secondly, the median BMI of our patient cohort was in the overweight category, so our findings may underrepresent what is in obese populations. Thirdly, we had female dominance, which is not consistent with societal statistics. Finally, our prediction model’s Nagelkerke R square value is not high. The relatively narrow interquartile range of age and BMI appears to be the cause of this.

We also acknowledge the strengths of the study. Firstly, to the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to demonstrate weight misperception and its characteristics among the Turkish adult population. This study was not an e-mail or phone-based study but rather conducted face-to-face with the researcher involved in the study. We believe that this approach prevented misunderstandings of the questions and provided more detailed and consistent answers. Moreover, we did not rush patients to answer the questions as observed in phone-based studies but allocated an abundant amount of time. Secondly, despite the study taking place in academic centers, the nature of general internal medicine means that patients admitted to these outpatient clinics primarily reflect the general public. Finally, we did not dichotomize misperceptions but rather grouped them under three main headings. This is an important classification because, as illustrated above, TTRM and FTRM misperceptions have different associations.

In conclusion, weight misperception is a common phenomenon among Turkish adult patients. Underestimation is more common than overestimation and is associated with older age, a higher body mass index, and lower educational status. The association between weight misperception and adverse health consequences necessitates further research with a larger population.

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

1. Zhang X, Ha S, Lau HCH, Yu J. Excess body weight: novel insights into its roles in obesity comorbidities. Semin Cancer Biol. 2023;92:16–27. doi:10.1016/j.semcancer.2023.03.008

2. Avgerinos KI, Spyrou N, Mantzoros CS, Dalamaga M. Obesity and cancer risk: emerging biological mechanisms and perspectives. Metabolism. 2019;92:121–135. doi:10.1016/j.metabol.2018.11.001

3. Chen Y, Dabbas W, Gangemi A, et al. Obesity management and chronic kidney disease. Semin Nephrol. 2021;41(4):392–402. doi:10.1016/j.semnephrol.2021.06.010

4. Borlaug BA, Jensen MD, Kitzman DW, Lam CSP, Obokata M, Rider OJ. Obesity and heart failure with preserved ejection fraction: new insights and pathophysiological targets. Cardiovasc Res. 2023;118(18):3434–3450. doi:10.1093/cvr/cvac120

5. Rojano A, Sena E, Manzano-Nuñez R, Pericàs JM, Ciudin A. NAFLD as the metabolic hallmark of obesity. Intern Emerg Med. 2023;18(1):31–41. doi:10.1007/s11739-022-03139-x

6. Ruze R, Liu T, Zou X, et al. Obesity and type 2 diabetes mellitus: connections in epidemiology, pathogenesis, and treatments. Front Endocrinol. 2023;14:1161521. doi:10.3389/fendo.2023.1161521

7. Powell-Wiley TM, Poirier P, Burke LE, et al. Obesity and cardiovascular disease: a scientific statement from the American heart association. Circulation. 2021;143(21):e984–e1010. doi:10.1161/CIR.0000000000000973

8. Chrysant SG. Pathophysiology and treatment of obesity-related hypertension. J Clin Hypertens. 2019;21(5):555–559. doi:10.1111/jch.13518

9. Perdomo CM, Cohen RV, Sumithran P, Clément K, Frühbeck G. Contemporary medical, device, and surgical therapies for obesity in adults. Lancet. 2023;401(10382):1116–1130. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(22)02403-5

10. Wiechert M, Holzapfel C. Nutrition concepts for the treatment of obesity in adults. Nutrients. 2021;14(1):169. doi:10.3390/nu14010169

11. Rawana JS. The relative importance of body change strategies, weight perception, perceived social support, and self-esteem on adolescent depressive symptoms: longitudinal findings from a national sample. J Psychosom Res. 2013;75(1):49–54. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2013.04.012

12. Liu BP, Jia CX, Li SX. The association of weight control attempts with suicidality: the role of objective weight status and weight perception among adolescents of United States. J Affect Disord. 2023;325:369–377. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2022.12.168

13. Roberts RE, Duong HT. Perceived weight, not obesity, increases risk for major depression among adolescents. J Psychiatr Res. 2013;47(8):1110–1117. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.03.019

14. Moorman EL, Warnick JL, Acharya R, Janicke DM. The use of internet sources for nutritional information is linked to weight perception and disordered eating in young adolescents. Appetite. 2020;154:104782. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2020.104782

15. Dey R, Clark BR, Ackermann N, Racette SB. Weight status perception and weight loss intention among urban youth. Obes Res Clin Pract. 2019;13(4):391–394. doi:10.1016/j.orcp.2019.04.004

16. Esenay FI, Sezer TA. Understanding the Turkish adolescents’ weight perception: a cross‐sectional study. J Child Adolesc Nurs. 2022;35(4):349–355. doi:10.1111/jcap.12389

17. Yang K, Turk MT, Allison VL, James KA, Chasens E. Body mass index self‐perception and weight management behaviors during late adolescence. J Sch Health. 2014;84(10):654–660. doi:10.1111/josh.12195

18. Joo YY, Kim J, Lee K, Cho GJ, Yi KW. Misperception of body weight and associated socioeconomic and health-related factors among Korean female adults: a nationwide population-based study. Front Endocrinol. 2022;13:1007129.

19. Althumiri NA, Basyouni MH, BinDhim NF, Alqahtani SA. Levels and associations of weight misperception with healthy lifestyle among adults in Saudi Arabia. Obes Facts. 2021;14(6):586–592. doi:10.1159/000518633

20. Sonneville KR, Thurston IB, Milliren CE, Gooding HC, Richmond TK. Weight misperception among young adults with overweight/obesity associated with disordered eating behaviors. Int J Eating Disorders. 2016;49(10):937–946. doi:10.1002/eat.22565

21. Lee K. effects of weight misperception on the association between BMI and self-rated health in midlife and elderly Koreans. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2022;100:104664. doi:10.1016/j.archger.2022.104664

22. Park S, Lee S, Hwang J, Kwon JW. The impact of weight misperception on health-related quality of life in Korean adults (KNHANES 2007–2014): a community-based cross-sectional study. BMJ Open. 2017;7(6):e016098. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2017-016098

23. Javed Z, Valero‐Elizondo J, Maqsood MH, et al. Social determinants of health and obesity: findings from a national study of US adults. Obesity. 2022;30(2):491–502. doi:10.1002/oby.23336

24. Baez AS, Ortiz-Whittingham LR, Tarfa H, et al. Social determinants of health, health disparities, and adiposity. Prog Cardiovasc Dis. 2023;78:17–26. doi:10.1016/j.pcad.2023.04.011

25. Harris CV, Bradlyn AS, Coffman J, Gunel E, Cottrell L. BMI-based body size guides for women and men: development and validation of a novel pictorial method to assess weight-related concepts. Int J Obes. 2008;32(2):336–342. doi:10.1038/sj.ijo.0803704

26. Wan Abdul Hamed WN, Abd Aziz NA. Barriers in adopting healthy body weight among Malaysian population: a cross-sectional study of body weight perception and misperception versus actual body weight. J Prim Care Community Health. 2020;11:215013272090747. doi:10.1177/2150132720907472

27. Akindele MO, Phillips J, Igumbor E, Useh U. Body weight misperception and dissatisfaction among overweight and obese adult Nigerians. JMIR Public Health Surveill. 2017;3(3):e56. doi:10.2196/publichealth.7047

28. Monteagudo Sánchez C, Dijkstra SC, Visser M. Self-perception of body weight status in older Dutch adults. J Nutr Health Aging. 2015;19(6):612–618. doi:10.1007/s12603-015-0486-z

29. Kye SY, Park K. Gender differences in factors associated with body weight misperception. Public Health Nutr. 2021;24(9):2483–2495. doi:10.1017/S1368980020003262

30. Caleyachetty R, Kengne AP, Muennig P, Rutter H, Echouffo-Tcheugui JB. Misperception of body weight among overweight or obese adults in Mauritius. Obes Res Clin Pract. 2016;10(2):216–219. doi:10.1016/j.orcp.2016.02.006

31. Draper CE, Davidowitz KJ, Goedecke JH. Perceptions relating to body size, weight loss and weight-loss interventions in black South African women: a qualitative study. Public Health Nutr. 2016;19(3):548–556. doi:10.1017/S1368980015001688

32. Rasheed P. Perception of body weight and self-reported eating and exercise behaviour among obese and non-obese women in Saudi Arabia. Public Health. 1998;112(6):409–414. doi:10.1016/S0033-3506(98)00288-1

33. Dunkel TM, Davidson D, Qurashi S. Body satisfaction and pressure to be thin in younger and older Muslim and non-Muslim women: the role of western and non-western dress preferences. Body Image. 2010;7(1):56–65. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2009.10.003

34. Lemon SC, Rosal MC, Zapka J, Borg A, Andersen V. Contributions of weight perceptions to weight loss attempts: differences by body mass index and gender. Body Image. 2009;6(2):90–96. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2008.11.004

35. Mokdad AH. The spread of the obesity epidemic in the United States, 1991-1998. JAMA. 1999;282(16):1519. doi:10.1001/jama.282.16.1519

36. Mosli HH, Kutbi HA, Alhasan AH, Mosli RH. Understanding the interrelationship between education, income, and obesity among adults in Saudi Arabia. Obes Facts. 2020;13(1):77–85. doi:10.1159/000505246

37. Chao AM, Quigley KM, Wadden TA. Dietary interventions for obesity: clinical and mechanistic findings. J Clin Invest. 2021;131(1). doi:10.1172/JCI140065

留言 (0)

沒有登入
gif