Brain network mechanism on cognitive control task in the elderly with brain aging: A functional near infrared spectroscopy study

1. Research background of cognitive control and brain aging

Cognitive control refers to the fundamental ability to ensure goal-oriented behaviors, including task switching, reaction inhibition and working memory. It is a broader concept than selective attention (Parris et al., 2022). Cognitive control participates in many traditional cognitive dimensions, including attention, execution and memory (Harada et al., 2013). People’s behavior is highly adaptable and flexible, and even if environmental factors interfere, people can make adjustments within a certain range and finally achieve their goals. Behind this flexibility is the role of cognitive control (Schuch et al., 2022). Selective attention and response inhibition in cognitive control can be effectively monitored by cognitive psychological monitoring methods (Braem et al., 2019; Mäki-Marttunen et al., 2019).

The related manifestations of cognitive control decreased will appear in the early stage of senile brain aging (Bettio et al., 2017). The decline of cognitive function will gradually develop into cognitive impairment-related diseases, such as mild cognitive impairment, Alzheimer’s disease (AD), dementia, etc., which will lead to the decline of life quality, aggravate neuropsychiatric symptoms and disabilities, and increase medical costs (Zheng et al., 2018). Early monitoring and intervention of cognition can effectively delay the processing of dementia and AD (Levin et al., 2010). Therefore, it is helpful for early screening and treatment to study the brain mechanism of cognitive control dysfunction caused by early brain aging (Zammit et al., 2016). In this study, functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) was used to monitor the dynamic changes of brain network in normal elderly people during judgment test, so as to explore the influence mechanism of age-induced brain degeneration on cognitive and motor areas of prefrontal cortex.

2. Experimental design 2.1. Research object

In this study, from January 2022 to April 2022, 20 normal elderly and 21 normal young people were recruited in the Fifth Affiliated Hospital of Guangzhou Medical University. Inclusion criteria for healthy elderly: ➀ age 50–75 years; ➁ no previous history of vital organ disease; ➂ no brain tumor or other brain disease; ➃ right-handed; ➄ normal intellectual and cognitive ability; ➅ no history of mental illness. Exclusion criteria: ➀ Severe cognitive impairment or mental illness, unable to cooperate with instructions or tests; ➁ skull defect, head infection and skin damage are impossible for near-infrared brain imaging. Inclusion criteria for healthy young adults: ➀ age 18–30 years; ➁ no previous history of vital organ disease; ➂ no brain tumor or other brain disease; ➃ right-handed; ➄ normal intellectual and cognitive ability; ➅ no history of mental illness. All subjects have signed the informed consent form for the experiment. The research scheme has been approved by the Administrative Committee of the Fifth Affiliated Hospital of Guangzhou Medical University (KY01-2021-03-20) and registered by China Clinical Trial Center (ChiCTR2200058088).

2.2. Experimental methods 2.2.1. Clinical evaluation

In this study, all subjects were assessed by Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) and fNIRS synchronous judgment test.

2.2.2. Experimental equipment and fNIRS scheme setting

The NirSmart (Danyang Huichuang, China), a portable fNIRS imaging device of the Huichuang, was used to collect the data of the subjects during the task. Two kinds of near-infrared (730, 850 nm) signals with different wavelengths were recorded in the form of continuous wave, and the sampling frequency was 11 Hz. The fNIRS acquisition head cap consists of 10 detectors and 12 transmitters, with a total of 24 channels distributed in regions of interest (ROI). The detectors and transmitters placing followed by the International 10–20 system. The ROI is set in bilateral prefrontal cortex (PFC) and primary motor cortex (PMC), as shown in Figure 1. The channel distribution adopts the positioning system Nirspace (Danyang Huichuang Medical Equipment Co., Ltd., Danyang, China) of Polhemus FASTRAK 3D digital instrument to locate the brain area. After entering the evaluation room, the subjects sat comfortably for 5 min, got familiar with the environment, and eliminated the tension. The fNIRS synchronous judgment test scheme is designed as Block. Firstly, the resting state of 10 s is collected as the baseline. Then, the task state during the trial was collected for 60 s, and then the rest state data was collected for 60 s. As a trial, the trial was repeated for three times, totaling 370 s.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. fNIRS observation channel distribution and ROI division diagram. RPMC, right primary motor cortex; LPMC, left primary motor cortex; RPFC, right prefrontal cortex; LPFC, left prefrontal cortex. PMC includes primary motor cortex, anterior motor cortex, and auxiliary motor cortex; PFC includes dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and frontal pole.

2.2.3. Judgment of experimental design

The judgment task paradigm adopted in this study is squared-block test, including forward test and reverse test. Subjects need to click the “←” or “→” keyboard button according to the test content and the position of the font on the screen in the squared-block. When judging the forward test, the subject are required to click the key in the same position as the font in the squared-block; In the reverse judgment test, the subject are required to click the key in the opposite position as the font in the squared-block. The picture of squared-block test is presented for 4,000 ms, and then the cross gaze point is presented for 2,000 ms, this is a complete stimulus. A trial consists of 10 stimulus, and three trials are conducted continuously, every test interval has a rest time of 60 s. After completion, the trial ends (Figure 2). Squared-block test is a kind of research on cognitive psychology, mainly involves executive control, attention, language, cognition and other fields, usually around the internal mechanism of Stroop effect, or as a trial means, to explore a variety of psychological and cognitive problems (Yoo et al., 2020).

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Flow chart of the judgment trial design.

2.2.4. fNIRS synchronous judgment test flow

After the researcher puts fNIRS on the subjects and determines the effectiveness of the channel, the subjects are instructed to adjust their sitting position. The forefinger and ring finger of the right hand are placed on the “←” and “→” keyboards respectively, and only clicking on the keyboard is required during the experiment. The experimental environment should be kept quiet all the time, and the limbs and head should not be moved at will.

At the beginning of the experiment, an instruction appeared, and the experimenter instructed the subjects to read the instruction and practice. The practice lasted for five complete stimuli, which ensured that the subjects understood the experimental process and could cooperate with the completion of the experiment. After the exercise, the formal test was started, and the forward and reverse judgment tests were carried out. fNIRS synchronously collected the hemoglobin changes of ROI through the marker. During this period, the key reaction time and accuracy rate of subjects were recorded. Carry out forward test first, and then reverse test.

2.3. fNIRS data processing 2.3.1. Use NirSpark software to pre-process fNIRS data

➀ Remove of time interval and motion artifacts; ➁ Converting of the original optical signal into an optical density curve; ➂ Band-pass filtering (0.1–0.2 Hz) to filter the physiological noise caused by heart rate, respiration, exercise and the physiological noise caused by motion artifacts (Tan et al., 2015). Overall, in order to remove the low-frequency elements from the original signal, a sixth-order Butterworth band-pass filter was used to obtain the 0.1–0.2 Hz filter signal. Then, the sliding averaging method is used to remove the signal noise to improve the signal-to-noise ratio (Huppert et al., 2009); ➃ Calculate the concentration of oxygenated hemoglobin and deoxyhaemoglobin by the Hemo module; ➄ Calculate the eigenvalues of the forward and backward judgment tests with Block design (Yang et al., 2022).

2.3.2. Functional connectivity (FC) analysis

Functional connectivity (FC) refers to the temporal correlation between distant neurophysiological events in space. Brain imaging technology can be used to detect the functional communication between multiple brain regions (Zheng et al., 2021). In the Network module of NirSpark software, the changes in HbO2 and HbR concentrations in subjects at each time point measured at rest were extracted, and the Pearson correlation coefficient of HbO2 and HbR contents in each channel in the time series was analyzed. Fisher Z-transformation was then performed, and the transformed value was defined as the FC strength between channels. FC data were processed by NirSpark software and corrected by false discovery rate (FDR).

Functional near-infrared spectroscopy (FNIRS) brain network data processing: ➀ Define ROI in the functional brain network analysis module as bilateral PFC area (4 channels left and right) and bilateral PMC area (8 channels left and right); ➁ Calculate the mean value of oxygenated hemoglobin signal in the ROI brain region; ➂ Calculate the functional connectivity (FC) data of homologous brain regions and heterologous brain regions.

2.4. Statistical analysis

SPSS 25.0 software was used for statistical analysis (Tak and Ye, 2014), and GraphPad 8.0 was used to draw data results. All subjects’ MMSE, FC value of brain network, reaction time of judgment test and correct rate data were tested normally, and all measurement data were expressed by mean standard deviation. The MMSE, the FC value of brain network, the correct rate of judgment test and the reaction time data between the two groups which accord with the normal distribution were tested by independent sample t-test, and comparison of data within the group were tested by paired t-test. For the data that does not conform to the normal distribution, Wilcoxon symbolic rank sum test should be used in intra-group statistics, and Mann-Whitney U test should be used in inter-group statistics. Statistical significance difference is defined as P < 0.05.

3. Results 3.1. General information

A total of 41 subjects were included in this study, including 21 young people with an average age of 21.476 ± 0.928 years; 20 elderly people, with an average age of 59.550 ± 8.401 years; The MMSE of the elderly group was significantly lower than that of the young group (Z = 3.389, P = 0.001), but all were in the normal range (see Table 1).

www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Comparison of general data of two groups of subjects.

3.2. Within-group and between-group differences in behavioral data for forward and reverse judgment tests

In the forward and reverse judgment tests, the reaction time of the elderly group was significantly longer than that of the young group (forward Z value = −5.269, P < 0.001; reverse Z value = −5.112, P < 0.001). However, there was no statistical difference between the two groups in the correct rate of forward and reverse tests. There was no statistical difference in the reaction time and the correct rate within the two groups of subjects in the forward and reverse tests (see Table 2 and Figure 3).

www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Comparison of reaction time and correct rate of judgment test between young and elderly.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Comparison between groups and within groups of behavioral data of two groups of subjects’ judgment test. (A,B) Are the comparison between two groups of subjects’ judgment test behavior data, panel (A) is the comparison between groups at reaction time, and panel (B) is the comparison between groups at correct rate. (C,D) Shows the intra-group comparison of behavioral data of two groups of subjects’ judgment test, panel (C) is the intra-group comparison of reaction time and panel (D) is the intra-group comparison of correct rate; as can be seen from panel (A), the reaction time of the elderly group is significantly longer than that of the young group in the forward and reverse judgment tests (P < 0.05). *P < 0.05; **P < 0.01; ***P < 0.001.

3.3. Differences between groups of brain networks in forward judgment test

In the comparison of homologous brain networks between the two groups, the FC values of LPMC (t = 3.647, P = 0.001), RPMC (Z = 2.426, P = 0.015), LPFC (t = 4.255, P < 0.001), and RPFC (Z = 3.678, P < 0.001) in the elderly group were significantly lower than those of the young group (see Table 3 and Figure 4A).

www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Comparison of functional connectivity (FC) values of homologous and heterologous brain networks between the two groups in forward judgment test.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Comparison between groups of FC of homologous and heterologous ROI in forward and reverse judgment test. (A,B) Shows the forward judgment test, panel (A) is the inter-group comparison of FC values of homologous ROI between young and elderly, and panel (B) is the inter-group comparison of heterologous ROI. (C,D) Shows the comparison between groups of homologous ROI and heterologous ROI in the reverse judgment test. It can be seen from panels (A,C) that the FC values of homologous ROI between the two groups are statistically different in the forward and reverse judgment tests (P < 0.05). Panel (B) shows that the FC values of LPMC-RPMC, LPMC-RPFC, RPMC-RPFC, RPMC-LPFC, and RPFC-LPFC in the elderly group are significantly lower than those in the young group (P < 0.05). It can be seen from panel (D) that the FC values of LPMC-RPFC, LPMC-LPFC and RPFC-LPFC in the elderly group are significantly lower than those in the young group (P < 0.05). FC, functional connectivity; LPFC, left prefrontal cortex; RPFC, right prefrontal cortex; LPMC, left primary motor cortex; RPMC, right primary motor cortex; ROI, region of interest; *P < 0.05; **P < 0.01; ***P < 0.001.

In the comparison of heterologous brain network between the two groups, the FC values of LPMC-RPMC (t = 3.727, P = 0.001), LPMC-RPFC (t = 2.668, P = 0.011), RPMC-RPFC (t = 2.988, P = 0.005), RPMC-LPFC (t = 2.737, P = 0.009), RPFC-LPFC (t = 4.473, P < 0.001) in the elderly group were significantly lower than those of the young group, and there was no statistical difference in LPMC-LPFC between the two groups (see Table 3 and Figure 4B).

3.4. Differences between groups of brain networks in reverse judgment experiment

In the comparison of homologous brain networks between the two groups, the FC values of LPMC (t = 2.609, P = 0.013), RPMC (Z = 2.008, P = 0.045), LPFC (t = 3.792, P < 0.001), and RPFC (Z = 3.284, P < 0.003) in the elderly group were significantly lower than those of the young group (see Table 4 and Figure 4C).

www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Comparison of functional connectivity (FC) values of homologous and heterologous brain networks between the two groups in reverse judgment test.

In the comparison of heterologous brain network between the two groups, the FC values of LPMC-RPFC (t = 2.413, P = 0.021), LPMC-LPFC (Z = 2.113, P = 0.035), and RPFC-LPFC (t = 4.260, P < 0.001) in the elderly group were significantly lower than those of the young group, and there was no statistical difference in LPMC-RPPMC, RPMC-RPFC, RPMC-LPFC between the two groups (see Table 4 and Figure 4D).

3.5. Intra-group differences of FC values of brain networks in forward and reverse judgment tests

In the comparison between forward and reverse test brain networks, the FC value of the heterologous brain network RPMC-RPFC in the elderly group in the reverse test was significantly higher than that in the forward test (t value = −2.764, P = 0.012), and FC values of the other homologous ROI and heterologous ROI had no statistical difference within the group. There was no statistical difference in the FC value of brain network of young people in the forward and reverse judgment tests (see Table 5 and Figures 5, 6).

www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Intra-group differences of functional connectivity (FC) values of brain network in forward and reverse judgment tests.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Intra-group comparison of FC of homologous and heterologous ROI of two groups of subjects. (A,B) Are young groups, panel (A) is the intra-group comparison of FC values of forward and reverse judgment tests of homologous ROI, and panel (B) is the intra-group comparison of heterologous ROI; (C,D) are the elderly group, panel (C) is the intra-group comparison of homologous ROI, and panel (D) is the intra-group comparison of heterologous ROI; panel (D) shows that the FC value of RPMC-RPFC is statistically different between the forward and reverse judgment test (P < 0.05). FC, functional connection; LPFC, left prefrontal cortex; RPFC, right prefrontal cortex; LPMC, left primary motor cortex; RPMC, right primary motor cortex; ROI, region of interest; *P < 0.05.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Brain network and FC matrix of two groups of subjects. (A,B,E,F) are young group, and (C,D,G,H) are elderly group; Panels (A–D) are forward judgment tests; Panels (E–H) are reverse judgment tests.

4. Discussion

Brain aging without neurodegenerative diseases, usually called non-pathological brain aging or normal cognitive aging, is characterized by impaired memory and cognitive function. With the increase of age, brain function gradually deteriorates, and age-related cognitive impairment accumulates, which increases the risk of neurodegenerative diseases (Borrás Blasco and Viña Ribes, 2016; Mattson and Arumugam, 2018). An important aspect of cognitive control is reaction inhibition, that is, information processing is performed correctly while interference information is suppressed. Reaction inhibition is affected by the gradual decline of cognitive ability in the process of aging (Tafuro et al., 2019). In this study, fNIRS synchronous judgment test was used to explore the mechanism of the influence of age on cognitive control behavior and brain network.

4.1. The influence of brain aging on judging test results

The forward judgment test only needs to perform the consistent key pressing task according to the picture presentation direction, which belongs to automatic processing, while the reverse judgment test needs to suppress the trend of processing in the same direction and then perform reverse processing. The results of this study show that the reaction time of the elderly group is significantly longer than that of the young group, but there is no statistical difference between the two groups in the correct rate. It should be noted that the MMSE scores of the elderly group are all at a normal level, suggesting that although the cognitive function is at a normal level, cognitive function has deteriorated in behavior, mainly in executive function, information processing, selective attention and reaction inhibition. Compared with young people, the reaction time of the old people’s judgment test is prolonged, and the consideration is caused by the slow speed of information processing. In the process of brain aging, the slowdown of information processing speed is a common manifestation. Yano (2011) conducted Simon test on the old and young people, the results showed that the reaction time of the old people was significantly longer than that of the young people. It was found that brain aging might reduce the speed of information processing, but it did not affect the inhibition of reaction, which was consistent with the behavioral results of this study. However, there is no obvious difference between the reaction time and the correct rate in the forward and reverse behavioral comparison of the old people’s judgment test. The reasons are as follows: ➀ The ability of automatic information processing is degraded. Information processing is mainly divided into automatic processing and controlled processing. Automatic processing does not require the participation of attention, while controlled processing needs extra attention (Li et al., 2022). The forward test belongs to automatic processing, while the reverse test belongs to controlled processing. As the elderly get older, their ability to process information automatically deteriorates, which may require longer reaction time to process information; ➁ Impairment of response inhibition function: manifested by the ability to inhibit irrelevant information decreases (Commodari and Guarnera, 2008; Nicosia and Balota, 2020); ➂ Setting the difficulty of judgment test: the difficulty and intensity of judgment test will affect cognitive control and cognitive reserve consumption; ➃ Attention deficit: Attention is the key ability to extract relevant information, and attention deficit can easily be interfered by other information, so that the target information cannot be accurately extracted (Zoccatelli et al., 2010). Judgment test can be used as a method to monitor the information processing speed of brain aging, but there is still no objective evidence to explore the mechanism of brain aging from behavior. Therefore, this study introduced synchronous fNIRS technology to explore the functional interaction between cognitive related brain regions.

4.2. The influence of brain aging on the brain network of cognitive control tasks

This study found that the FC of homologous ROI in the elderly group was weaker than that in the young group in the forward and reverse judgment tests, which indicated that the FC of bilateral PFC and PMC decreased in the elderly group during cognitive tasks, suggesting that the function of brain network in cognitive and motor-related brain regions was weakened. Zhang et al. (2021) proposed that the increase of age was negatively related to the FC of the whole brain network, while the decrease of the FC of the brain network was related to cognitive behavior disorder, which supported the experimental results of this study. A large number of studies have shown that the FC values of age-related areas decrease in the default mode network, salience network and frontoparietal control network (Grady, 2012). Age-related functional brain network changes accelerate cognitive decline, and brain functional reorganization related to brain aging may be the key sign of cognitive aging (Zhao et al., 2020). Li et al. (2018) conducted fNIRS resting whole brain analysis on young people and old people. The results showed that the whole brain and local brain functions of old people decreased, and the results of small world analysis also showed a downward trend. However, young people showed rich hubs in prefrontal cortex, while the display hubs of old people shifted to sensory motor cortex. Therefore, brain aging not only affects local brain regions, but also affects the interaction of brain network functions among brain regions.

4.3. The influence of brain aging on pFC-related brain network

The FC value of heterologous ROI mainly reflects the FC strength of brain network between different ROI brain regions. The decrease of brain network connection strength between different brain regions leads to the decrease of cognitive control, which is the expression of the loss of overall integration function (Bishop et al., 2010). In this study, it was found that in the forward judgment test, except LPMC-LPFC, the heterologous ROI of PMC and PFC in the elderly group was significantly lower than that in the young group. However, in the reverse judgment test, only the heterogenous ROI of LPMC-RPFC, LPMC-LPFC, and RPFC-LPFC in the elderly group was significantly lower than that in the young group, which indicated that the interaction function of most PFC and PMC brain networks decreased in the forward judgment test, while that of both PFC and LPMC brain networks decreased in the reverse judgment test, suggesting that the interaction between PFC and PMC brain networks decreased in the simple cognitive task of the elderly, but there was a certain compensatory ability after the task difficulty was increased. Judgment task is a classic paradigm that is most often used to study top-down cognitive control processing, and it is mainly used to study selective attention and response inhibition function (Aguerre et al., 2021). Top-down cognitive control processing is accomplished by mobilizing, coordinating and directing subcortical and posterior cortical areas through the core carrier PFC (Aguerre et al., 2021). Sirawaj Itthipuripat et al. (2019) have found that people’s information processing is highly automated when people look at familiar stimuli, but PFC is a key brain area that plays a role in cognitive interference ability and reducing the trend of high automation. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) shows that the functional changes of prefrontal lobe are related to normal brain aging (Cabeza et al., 2018). A large number of studies have shown that the aging of frontal lobe is related to the deterioration of executive function, indicating that frontal lobe is crucial to cognitive control (Fisher et al., 2014; Cipolotti et al., 2015; Rieck et al., 2021). Zheng et al. (2014) found that the right inferior frontal gyrus was related to the response inhibition task. In this study, it was found that only the heterologous brain network RPMC-RPFC in the elderly group in the reverse judgment test was significantly stronger than in the forward judgment test, and there was no significant difference in the FC of each brain network channel in the left ROI, showing a tendency of lateralization, suggesting that the enhancement of RPMC-RPFC brain network connection may be related to cognitive compensation. Yeung and Chan (2021) have shown that aging is related to the increased compensatory activity of frontal lobe in multiple task areas. The activation of PFC-related network on the right side may be related to visual processing, Brosnan et al. (2018) have found that the activation of PFC on the right side is related to the processing speed of visual information, which may be a sign of age-related decline. Therefore, the brain network interaction mode between PFC and other brain regions in the elderly has certain brain aging characteristics.

5. Limitations and prospects

This study explores the characteristics of brain network of brain aging from the cognitive perspective, and provides a direction for follow-up research. However, this study failed to cover more brain regions to explore the FC mechanism of brain network. In the future research, it is hoped that the research scope of brain regions can be further expanded, and the longitudinal research on the improvement mechanism of brain aging can be integrated to further explore the functional remodeling mechanism of brain aging, so as to promote the targeted assessment and rehabilitation of degenerative diseases of the elderly, and achieve early diagnosis, early treatment and early rehabilitation.

6. Summary

In this study, fNIRS synchronous judgment test was used to reveal the brain network mechanism of cognitive control of brain aging. It was found that the FC of brain network of the elderly in bilateral PFC and PMC was significantly lower than that of the young people, and there might be a tendency of lateralization, suggesting that brain aging is a kind of deterioration of whole brain function, which can reduce the speed of information processing and form a connection mode of brain network function different from that of the young people.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in this study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding authors.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Administrative Committee of the Fifth Affiliated Hospital of Guangzhou Medical University (KY01-2021-03-20) and registered by China Clinical Trial Center (ChiCTR2200058088). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

JL, HO, and WL designed the study. JL, JH, and WL drafted the manuscript. JL, WL, YW, and ZT performed data analysis. WL, YW, JL, ZL, and LZ collected the data. JL and HO approved the final version of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was supported by Guangzhou Health Science Technology Project (20211A011105); 2019 Young Innovative Talents Projects in Ordinary Colleges and Universities in Guangdong Province (2019KQNCX119); Guangdong Medical Research Fund Project (B2021007); Guangzhou Basic Research Program, the Municipal Schools (Institutes) Jointly Funded Basic Projects (202102010100); Key Medical Disciplines in Guangzhou (2021–2023); and Key Laboratory of Biological Targeted Diagnosis and Rehabilitation in Guangdong University (2021KSYS009).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Aguerre, N., Bajo, M., and Gómez-Ariza, C. (2021). Dual mechanisms of cognitive control in mindful individuals. Psychol. Res. 85, 1909–1921. doi: 10.1007/s00426-020-01377-2.a

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bettio, L., Rajendran, L., and Gil-Mohapel, J. (2017). The effects of aging in the hippocampus and cognitive decline. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 79, 66–86. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.04.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borrás Blasco, C., and Viña Ribes, J. (2016). Neurofisiología y envejecimiento. Concepto y bases fisiopatológicas del deterioro cognitivo [Neurophysiology and ageing. Definition and pathophysiological foundations of cognitive impairment]. Rev. Esp. Geriatr. Gerontol. 51, 3–6. doi: 10.1016/S0211-139X(16)30136-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braem, S., Bugg, J., Schmidt, J., Crump, M., Weissman, D., Notebaert, W., et al. (2019). Measuring adaptive control in conflict tasks. Trends Cogn. Sci. 23, 769–783.

Google Scholar

Brosnan, M., Demaria, G., Petersen, A., Dockree, P., Robertson, I., and Wiegand, I. (2018). Plasticity of the right-lateralized cognitive reserve network in ageing. Cereb. Cortex 28, 1749–1759. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabeza, R., Albert, M., Belleville, S., Craik, F., Duarte, A., Grady, C., et al. (2018). Maintenance, reserve and compensation: The cognitive neuroscience of healthy ageing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 19, 701–710. doi: 10.1038/s41583-018-0068-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cipolotti, L., Healy, C., Chan, E., MacPherson, S., White, M., Woollett, K., et al. (2015). The effect of age on cognitive performance of frontal patients. Neuropsychologia 75, 233–241. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.06.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huppert, T., Diamond, S., Franceschini, M., and Boas, D. (2009). HomER: A review of time-series analysis methods for near-infrared spectroscopy of the brain. Appl. Opt. 48, 280–298. doi: 10.1364/ao.48.00d280

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Itthipuripat, S., Deering, S., and Serences, J. (2019). When conflict cannot be avoided: Relative contributions of early selection and frontal executive control in mitigating stroop conflict. Cereb. Cortex 29, 5037–5048. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhz042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levin, O., Yunishchenko, N., and Dudarova, M. (2010). Efficacy of akatinol memantine in moderate cognitive impairments. Neurosci. Behav. Physiol. 40, 926–933. doi: 10.1007/s11055-010-9347-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, L., Babawale, O., Yennu, A., Trowbridge, C., Hulla, R., Gatchel, R., et al. (2018). Whole-cortical graphical networks at wakeful rest in young and older adults revealed by functional near-infrared spectroscopy. Neurophotonics 5:035004. doi: 10.1117/1.NPh.5.3.035004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, T., Zheng, Y., Wang, Z., Zhu, D., Ren, J., Liu, T., et al. (2022). Brain information processing capacity modeling. Sci. Rep. 12:2174. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-05870-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mäki-Marttunen, V., Hagen, T., and Espeseth, T. (2019). Proactive and reactive modes of cognitive control can operate independently and simultaneously. Acta Psychol. 199:102891. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2019.102891

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mattson, M., and Arumugam, T. (2018). Hallmarks of brain aging: Adaptive and pathological modification by metabolic states. Cell Metab. 27, 1176–1199. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2018.05.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parris, B., Hasshim, N., Wadsley, M., Augustinova, M., and Ferrand, L. (2022). The loci of Stroop effects: A critical review of methods and evidence for levels of processing contributing to color-word Stroop effects and the implications for the loci of attentional selection. Psychol. Res. 86, 1029–1053. doi: 10.1007/s00426-021-01554-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rieck, J., Baracchini, G., and Grady, C. (2021). Contributions of brain function and structure to three different domains of cognitive control in normal aging. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1–22. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01685 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schuch, S., Philipp, A., Maulitz, L., and Koch, I. (2022). On the reliability of behavioral measures of cognitive control: Retest reliability of task-inhibition effect, task-preparation effect, Stroop-like interference, and conflict adaptation effect. Psychol. Res. 86, 2158–2184. doi: 10.1007/s00426-021-01627-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tafuro, A., Ambrosini, E., Puccioni, O., and Vallesi, A. (2019). Brain oscillations in cognitive control: A cross-sectional study with a spatial stroop task. Neuropsychologia 133:107190. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.107190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tan, Q., Zhang, M., Wang, Y., Zhang, M., Wang, Y., Xin, Q., et al. (2015). Frequency-specific functional connectivity revealed by wavelet-based coherence analysis in elderly subjects with cerebral infarction using NIRS method. Med. Phys. 42, 5391–5403. doi: 10.1118/1.4928672

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, Z., Zhang, W., Liu, D., Zhang, S.-S., Tang, Y., Song, J., et al. (2022). Effects of sport stacking on neuropsychological, neurobiological, and brain function performances in patients with mild Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment: A randomized controlled trial. Front. Aging Neurosci. 14:910261. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.910261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yano, M. (2011). Aging effects in response inhibition: General slowing without decline in inhibitory functioning. J. Hum. Ergol. 40, 129–139.

Google Scholar

Yeung, M., and Chan, A. S. A. (2021). Systematic review of the application of functional near-infrared spectroscopy to the study of cerebral hemodynamics in healthy aging. Neuropsychol. Rev. 31, 139–166. doi: 10.1007/s11065-020-09455-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoo, S., Woo, S., Shin, M., Yoon, J., Shin, Y., and Hong, K. (2020). Diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment using cognitive tasks: A functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. Curr. Alzheimer Res. 17, 1145–1160. doi: 10.2174/1567205018666210212154941

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zammit, A., Katz, M., Bitzer, M., and Lipton, R. (2016). Cognitive impairment and dementia in older adults with chronic kidney disease: A review. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 30, 357–366. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0000000000000178

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, H., Gertel, V., Cosgrove, A., and Diaz, M. (2021). Age-related differences in resting-state and task-based network characteristics and cognition: A lifespan sample. Neurobiol. Aging 101, 262–272. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2020.10.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, J., Manza, P., Wiers, C., Song, H., Zhuang, P., Gu, J., et al. (2020). Age-related decreases in interhemispheric resting-state functional connectivity and their relationship with executive function. Front. Aging Neurosci. 12:20. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2020.00020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zheng, D., Sun, H., Dong, X., Liu, B., Xu, Y., Chen, S., et al. (2014). Executive dysfunction and gray matter atrophy in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiol. Aging 35, 548–555. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zheng, G., Zheng, Y., Xiong, Z., Ye, B., Tao, J., and Chen, L. (2018). Effect of Baduanjin exercise on cognitive function in patients with post-stroke cognitive impairment: Study protocol for a randomised controlled trial. BMJ Open 8:e020954. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2017-020954

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zheng, X., Luo, J., Deng, L., Li, B., Li, L., Huang, D., et al. (2021). Detection of functional connectivity in the brain during visuo-guided grip force tracking tasks: A functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. J. Neurosci. Res. 99, 1108–1119. doi: 10.1002/jnr.24769

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zoccatelli, G., Beltramello, A., Alessandrini, F., Pizzini, F., and Tassinari, G. (2010). Word and position interference in stroop tasks: A behavioral and fMRI study. Exp. Brain Res. 207, 139–147. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2433-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

留言 (0)

沒有登入
gif