C-low threshold mechanoreceptor activation becomes sufficient to trigger affective pain in spinal cord-injured mice in association with increased respiratory rates

1. Introduction

Neuropathic pain, a common type of pain arising from direct damage to the nervous system, is a clinically relevant outcome of spinal cord injury (SCI) that may be experienced at the level of injury (i.e., “at-level” pain; Finnerup et al., 2001; Siddall et al., 2003; Felix et al., 2007). Despite the prevalence of pain in approximately 70% of SCI patients (Siddall and Loeser, 2001), pharmacological treatments are often inadequate, only slightly reducing pain intensity (Baastrup and Finnerup, 2008). Although prior studies have focused primarily on plasticity within the injured spinal cord, there is increasing recognition that peripheral plasticity contributes to pain after SCI. For example, increased spontaneous activity in primary nociceptors generates pain (Bedi et al., 2010), and peripheral nociceptive input exacerbates pain behavior (Garraway et al., 2014; Martin et al., 2019), after SCI. While these previous studies identify activity in nociceptors as crucial to pain hypersensitivity, the role of specific afferent subpopulations, including non-nociceptors, in SCI-induced neuropathic pain remains poorly understood.

C-low threshold mechanoreceptors (C-LTMRs) may represent one group of primary afferents that contribute to pain after SCI, as they have been implicated in other injury models (Seal et al., 2009; Mahns and Nagi, 2013). C-LTMRs are small diameter, unmyelinated afferents that innervate the trunk hairy skin. They terminate in lamina II of the dorsal horn (Li et al., 2011), projecting onward to wide dynamic range (WDR) lamina I spinoparabrachial projection neurons (Andrew, 2010; Craig, 2010). C-LTMRs are defined by tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) expression (Li et al., 2011; Lou et al., 2013), and normally encode the affective component of pleasurable, affiliative touch (Iggo, 1960; Bessou et al., 1971; Olausson et al., 2002; Loken et al., 2009; Li et al., 2011; Liljencrantz and Olausson, 2014; Zimmerman et al., 2014). In SCI patients with ongoing pain, gentle brush stimuli applied to hairy skin at the lesion level, a stimulus associated with activation of C-tactile fiber (CTs, the equivalent of C-LTMRs in humans; Loken et al., 2009) produced hyperesthesia and allodynia (Finnerup et al., 2003). Thus, C-LTMRs are an important target for chronic pain research, but few preclinical studies have related SCI-induced pain to cutaneous trunk signaling, including afferent plasticity at the segmental level of injury (e.g., Oatway et al., 2004; Yezierski et al., 2004; Crown et al., 2006; Bedi et al., 2010) where these afferents reside. C-LTMRs may activate sympathetic pathways as part of the allodynic response. Conversely, sympathetic nerve stimulation directly excites C-LTMRs and increases their sensitivity to applied mechanical stimuli (Roberts and Levitt, 1982; Roberts and Elardo, 1985; Barasi and Lynn, 1986). Injury-induced pain is associated with increases in sympathetic activity (Kinnman and Levine, 1995a, b; Jänig et al., 1996; Ramer et al., 1999; Zhang et al., 2004), and nociceptive stimuli can activate the sympathetic nervous system, increasing heart rate and respiratory rate (RR; Wolf and Hardy, 1941; Culman et al., 1997; Loggia et al., 2011; Santuzzi et al., 2013). Slowing RR (Grant and Rainville, 2009; Noble et al., 2017) or blocking sympathetic pathways (Pinheiro et al., 2015) has been shown to mitigate pain sensitivity.

Our laboratory recently found that mechanical stimulation within the tuning range of C-LTMRs, delivered to adult rats after SCI, increased RRs at timepoints consistent with the expression of pain (Noble et al., 2019). SCI rats also had higher basal RRs acutely after injury. These results support the involvement of C-LTMRs in at-level SCI pain and suggest that acute changes in RRs may serve as a physiological index of pain. It is conceivable that C-LTMRs undergo central or peripheral plasticity after SCI. To identify the specific role C-LTMRs play in SCI-induced neuropathic pain, we undertook studies in adult transgenic mice using a novel conditioned place aversion (CPA) paradigm. We used optogenetic techniques to enable selective stimulation of TH-expressing cutaneous afferents in the trunk skin, and study its effects on affective-motivational (deemed “affective”) pain behavior and RRs after SCI.

2. Materials and Methods 2.1 Animal care and generation of transgenic mice

All experiments were performed in adult male and female transgenic mice expressing Cre recombinase under the regulatory control of the mouse TH gene, i.e., “TH-Cre” mice. Mice were approximately 10 weeks old and weighed 20–22 g (females) and 24–26 g (males) at the commencement of experimental procedures. They were housed in standard cages in a vivarium on a 12:12-h light-dark cycle (7 am lights on/7 pm lights off), with all behavioral testing and respiratory recordings performed during the light period. Animals were fed standard rodent diets ad libitum. Experimental procedures were approved by the Animal Care and Use Committee of Emory University and conformed to national standards for the care and use of experimental animals and the American Physiological Society’s “Guiding Principles in the Care and Use of Animals”.

Mice bred in our animal colony originated from one of two commercially available strains; Mutant Mouse Resource and Research Centers (MMRRC) #017262 or Jackson Laboratory (JAX) #025614 mice; the latter strain expresses a tamoxifen-inducible Cre recombinase (Cre/ERT2). For optogenetic experiments, the TH-Cre and tamoxifen-inducible “TH-Cre-ER” mice were crossed with a strain expressing a Cre-dependent channelrhodopsin (ChR)-2–eYFP (yellow fluorescent protein) fusion protein following exposure to Cre recombinase [Ai32(RCL-ChR2(H134R)/eYFP; JAX #024109], generating TH::ChR2-eYFP transgenic mice. This allows for direct optical targeting of C-LTMRs based on their known molecular profile (Li et al., 2011). The tamoxifen-inducible Cre mice crosses of strain TH::ChR2-eYFP were treated with tamoxifen dissolved in peanut oil [(TAM), 5 mg/day for 2 days (1 day apart) or 2 mg/day over three consecutive days), by subcutaneous (SC) administration at the scruff of the neck] to induce transgenic expression. TAM was administered at or shortly after weaning, at around 3 weeks of age, under light isoflurane anesthesia. This dosing regimen was chosen based on preliminary electrophysiology and immunohistochemistry experiments (e.g., Figure 1A) that provided confirmation of Cre-driven YFP expression.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Validation of TH-transgenic mice and SCI model of chronic neuropathic pain. (A) Left and top middle: co-expression of YFP and pERK in DRG ipsilateral (R-DRG) but not contralateral (L-DRG) to the side of optical stimulation (note pERK in red and YFP in green). White arrows indicate co-expression in individual cells. Bottom middle: representative immunohistochemical image of a thoracic (T) level 10 spinal cord section in one naïve TH::tdTomato adult mouse, demonstrating red fluorescent protein indicative of TH expression in superficial laminae of the spinal cord dorsal horn. Observed projection patterns support the recruitment of TH-positive C-LTMRs in the spinal cord (Li et al., 2011). Right: electrophysiological recordings validated successful afferent activation in a TH::ChR-eYFP mouse skin-nerve preparation. Activation was obtained from all nerves following trains of optogenetic stimulation (2.5 V, 5 Hz stim., ten 5-ms pulses). (B) The graph shows changes in mechanical reactivity to von Frey stimulation in SCI and sham control mice (N = 10 sham, N = 10 SCI). SCI mice showed mechanical hypersensitivity at 3–4 weeks post-injury, with significantly reduced hindpaw withdrawal thresholds compared to their own baseline values and sham mouse thresholds at the same chronic timepoints. On average, SCI mice withdrawal thresholds were reduced by 61% compared to sham controls. *p < 0.05 vs. Sham or BL, as indicated. (C) Differences in withdrawal threshold between chronic and baseline timepoints are plotted for individual mice with mean values superimposed (Sham and SCI), illustrating the basis of group differences. BL, baseline.

2.2 Surgical procedures and recovery

Mice were deeply anesthetized with isoflurane (5%, gas; lowered to 2%–3% once stable anesthesia was achieved). Under sterile conditions, a skin incision and dorsal laminectomy exposed the underlying spinal cord at lower thoracic level 10 (T10). For midline contusion injuries, mice received a 70 kdyne, zero dwell time, impact onto the dorsal surface of the spinal cord with an Infinite Horizon impactor (IH-0400 Impactor, Precision Systems and Instrumentation, Fairfax Station, VA, USA) as we previously described (Parvin et al., 2021; Martin et al., 2022). Care was taken to ensure that dorsal roots were not damaged by the laminectomy or impact, and on-target bilateral bruising of the dorsal spinal cord was verified by examination under a dissecting microscope. The overlying muscle and skin were sutured and the wound area treated with triple antibiotic ointment (bacitracin-neomycin-polymyxin B) topically. Sham control mice underwent the same surgical procedure but without receiving SCI. All mice (SCI and sham) were left to recover on a heated pad. Animals were given meloxicam (5 mg/kg, SC) immediately after surgery. They were also administered Lactated Ringer’s solution [0.5 ml, intraperitoneally (IP)] immediately after surgery, and an identical injection of 0.9% sterile saline daily for the first 48 h after surgery, to maintain hydration. Subsequent administration of saline was given only as needed. Mice received the antibiotic Baytril (2.5 mg/kg, SC) immediately after surgery and daily each morning up to 7 days post operation (dpo) to minimize the risk of urinary tract or bladder infection in SCI animals. Experimenters manually expressed mouse bladders twice daily for the duration of experiments or until animals had empty bladders for three consecutive days. Mice were weighed daily for 1 week after surgery and were subsequently weighed weekly and on days when behavioral tests were undertaken. Throughout the experiments, they were carefully monitored for signs of infection or distress. Mice were assessed for impairment of locomotor function at 1 dpo using the Basso Mouse Scale (BMS; Basso et al., 2006), to ensure the effectiveness of the injury. SCI mice were only included in the study if they recorded BMS scores of 0 or 1 at 1 dpo. Three mice (2 SCI and 1 sham) either died before the end of behavioral data collection or were SCI mice presenting with BMS scores greater than 1 at 1 dpo and were therefore excluded from subsequent analysis (also see “3. Results” Section).

2.3 Behavioral assays 2.3.1 von Frey test

At baseline (before surgery) and either 21 or 28 dpo (see below), a subset of mice was transferred to acrylic chambers in a behavioral testing room for the von Frey test of mechanical sensitivity. This assay was not performed 1 day after surgery since SCI mice had BMS scores of 0 or 1, indicating very little hindpaw placement. All mice were acclimated to the behavioral suite and testing apparatuses for at least 3 days prior to surgery. On testing days, following a 30-min acclimation period, individual animals were assayed for mechanical sensitivity (paw withdrawal responses) according to the established up-down method (Chaplan et al., 1994). Calibrated von Frey hairs (NC12775-99, North Coast Medical, Inc., Morgan Hill, CA, USA) starting with filament evaluator size 3.22 (target force 0.16 g) were administered from below a metal mesh platform to test each animal’s sensitivity to mechanical stimulation of the hindpaw. Right and left paw withdrawal thresholds were averaged to determine overall mechanical sensitivity. A reduction in von Frey withdrawal threshold values from their baseline levels corresponded to increased mechanical sensitivity.

2.3.2 Conditioned Place Aversion (CPA)

To provide a validated assessment of at-level, C-LTMR-mediated pain after SCI, our laboratory developed a modified CPA paradigm to model affective pain in mice using protocols adapted from previous studies (Hummel et al., 2008; Yang et al., 2014; Bagdas et al., 2016; Refsgaard et al., 2016; Wu et al., 2017) including our own (Martin et al., 2022). The custom-built CPA apparatus consisted of black and white chambers separated by a small partition. Each CPA box contained a small window permitting entry of a brush or fiber optic cable for optogenetic stimulation. Two separate experiments were conducted. The first investigated more ethologically relevant brush stimulation, with results identifying a time window of peak behavioral sensitivity after injury for targeting C-LTMRs with optogenetic techniques in the second experiment. In both experiments, stimuli were delivered across the animal’s trunk for a distance of ~3 cm, and stimulations were provided with an interstimulus interval of 30–60 s. Video recordings were collected throughout testing periods, and the percentage of time spent in the non-stimulated chamber before and after stimulation was taken to indicate relative place aversion. Note that for brush CPA, all mice were stimulated in the dark chamber, whereas to improve experimental design the initially preferred chamber (occasionally the light chamber) was always used as the stimulation chamber for optical CPA; hence the use of % in light (brush CPA) vs. % in non-preferred (optical CPA) chambers as reported outcome variables. For brush CPA, stimulation was undertaken at weekly timepoints to establish a preliminary timeline of C-LTMR-related pain aversion. Subsequently, results from these studies were used to guide the selection of timepoints for a 5-day CPA paradigm tailored to peak stimulus aversiveness for optical stimulation in the second experiment (separate mice).

2.3.2.1 Brush stimulation CPA cohort

This experiment was done in 14 TH-Cre mice (N = 7 each, SCI and sham). Using a modified light-dark chamber CPA paradigm, we assessed preferences at timepoints ranging from 1 day to 5 weeks after surgery. Starting 1 week after surgery, mice were given truncal stimulation with a small histology brush (Camel hair #4, Ted Pella, Inc., Redding, CA, USA) to study pain affect. Each 30-min test consisted of three distinct periods (compare this to the prolonged nature of optical CPA below, where in order to permit time for greater conditioned responses to develop, these three periods were spaced out over 5 days with no more than one occurring on a given day). During the pre-stimulation test, mice were given free access to both chambers (dark and light) for 10 min. Then, mice received 5 min of mechanical stimulation to the trunk with the brush (once/min), while confined to the dark chamber. For this, we chose a manual stimulation speed (~1 cm/s) that corresponded to the tuning properties of C-LTMRs (Loken et al., 2009; Noble et al., 2019). During a counterbalanced 5-min “control” epoch in the light chamber, mice received no mechanical stimulation. Finally, during the 10-min post-stimulation test, mice were again permitted access to both chambers for 10 min with no stimulation. Mice in this experiment underwent von Frey testing at baseline and 21 dpo.

2.3.2.2 Optogenetic stimulation CPA cohort

This experiment was done in 16 mice as follows: 12 TH-Cre-ER mice (N = 7 SCI, N = 5 sham) and four TH-Cre mice (N = 2 SCI, N = 2 sham), both the ChR crosses. Results were combined for presentation as there were no statistical differences between genotypes, so they are collectively referred to as TH::ChR2-eYFP mice. To encapsulate the period of peak sensitivity observed with a brush, centered at 28 dpo (see “3. Results” Section), the optical CPA test ran from 26 to 30 dpo. For the pre-conditioning test (26 dpo), mice were placed in the light chamber and given free access to the full apparatus for a 30-min test period to determine baseline time spent in each chamber. They then received daily 30-min conditioning sessions over the course of three days (27–29 dpo). In their preferred chambers, animals received optical stimulation (once/min for 10 min, after a 5-min habituation period) provided with a laser to activate C-LTMRs. A fiber optic cable was positioned close to the abdomen of mice and blue light illumination swept across the skin while animals were partially restrained. For partial restraint, mice were moved to an acrylic cylindrical tube (Rodent Restrainer; IITC Life Science, Woodland Hills, CA) that provided them with adequate room to move forwards and backwards but not sideways. This setup balanced the confinement necessary for high-fidelity electric field sensor recordings with stress reduction measures. Mice were acclimated to the cylinder for at least 3 days prior to surgery, and RR was confirmed to stabilize. Fur was shaved at the segmental level of injury for better illumination of cutaneous and subcutaneous nerve fibers. In the control, non-preferred chambers, mice received fake “stimulation” delivered with the laser box powered off but under similar partial restraint (again at once/min for 10 min after a 5-min habituation). The frequency, duration, and intensity of the light stimulation were manually controlled. The fiber optic cable delivered up to 5,204 mW/cm2 of blue light to the skin along the trunk. One day after the completion of the conditioning phase (at 30 dpo), each mouse again was allowed free access to explore both chambers for a 30-min post-conditioning test with no stimuli present. Mice in this experiment underwent hindpaw von Frey testing at baseline and 28 dpo, to verify that hindpaw sensitivity was evident at the time of CPA behavioral assessment.

2.4 Respiratory monitoring and analysis

Resting and optically-evoked respiratory measurements were obtained in a subset of mice used in the behavioral assays above. At baseline, and 1 and 21 dpo (with a subset at 7 dpo), mice were sequentially moved to a cylindrical tube (Rodent Restrainer, IITC Life Science, Woodland Hills, CA, USA) for 1-h respiratory recording sessions to establish basal, spontaneous (resting) RR. In mice receiving optical CPA, the longer duration of training during conditioning sessions (days 2–4 of the 5-day paradigm) as compared to brush CPA also permitted continuous monitoring of evoked RR. In both cases, recordings were obtained using remote sensors developed for this purpose.

2.4.1 Resting RR

Data from the 1-h recording sessions was used to determine resting RR values for baseline and several post-surgery testing points. Prior to experimentation, non-contact electric field sensors (EPIC, Plessey Semiconductors, Plymouth, UK) were affixed externally to the sides of rodent enclosures, with their wires connected to a power supply box. This box also adapted connections to Bayonet Neill–Concelman outputs for subsequent signal digitization and data collection. The analog sensor signal was low-pass filtered (bandwidth DC to 12 Hz) and digitized at unity gain and a sample rate of 1 kHz. The digitized data were continuously output to a Windows computer running LabVIEW (National Instruments, Austin, TX, USA), where recorded data were processed by a customized interface (program designed by William N. Goolsby). All manual and automated analysis of raw sensor output was accomplished using Clampfit analysis software (Molecular Devices, San Jose, CA, USA). In Clampfit, raw signals were analyzed and filtered, and threshold-based detection of individual breaths was performed. Clampfit calculated instantaneous RRs over the entire recording period, which were then averaged over periods of rest to determine a final value for resting RR. The first 20 min of each recording period, during animal acclimation, were left unscored. Video recordings verified that final values exclusively captured RR during animal resting states. Although it was attempted, recording RR during von Frey stimulation proved unreliable due to excessive motion artifacts.

2.4.2 Optically-evoked RR

Respiratory recordings were collected non-invasively during CPA procedures (optogenetic and control stimulation) using the remote sensors described above. Similar to resting RR, optically-evoked RR was calculated in Clampfit from scatterplots of instantaneous RR—in this case, concomitant with conditioning sessions in the CPA chamber (27–29 dpo). Approximately 10-s epochs of high-fidelity respiratory recordings were isolated from each interstimulus interval (i.e., between consecutive stimulations). Several replicate measurements of RR were then averaged to obtain individual data points for all six scorable time periods (the three conditioning sessions, with each session comprised of laser stimulation and control conditions).

2.5 Fluorescent histochemical and electrophysiological validation

To: (i) confirm that optical stimulation activated TH-positive cells in the dorsal root ganglia (DRG), and (ii) validate the expression pattern of TH projections in the spinal cord, fluorescent histochemistry was performed. To: (i) validate optical activation of the C-LTMRs, TH-Cre-ER mice were crossed with channelrhodopsin mice (JAX #024109) and the progeny (TH::ChR2-eYFP mice) were treated with TAM as described above. Two TAM-treated TH::ChR2-eYFP SCI mice were optically stimulated for 10 min in a similar manner to CPA conditioning sessions above, but to one side of the animal’s midline, to determine the effect of stimulation treatment on markers of neuronal activation. Mice were perfused 30 min following stimulation for immunolabeling, with cellular co-labeling of phosphorylated ERK (pERK) protein and TH (C-LTMRs, represented by YFP) shown in the DRG (Figure 1A, left and top middle). pERK is a key marker of neuronal activity and pain hypersensitivity (Xu et al., 2008; Gao and Ji, 2009; Garraway et al., 2011). To: (ii) reveal the expression pattern of TH projections in the spinal cord, TH-Cre-ER mice were crossed with tdTomato mice (JAX #007908) and the progeny (TH::tdTomato mice) were treated with TAM as described above. Two naïve TH::tdTomato mice were imaged (Figure 1A, bottom middle).

In both cases, mice were anesthetized with urethane (1.2 g/kg, IP) and transcardially perfused with phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) followed by 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) in PBS. The spinal cord and/or DRGs were dissected and post-fixed in 4% PFA for 2 h before being transferred to 30% sucrose for cryoprotection. Transverse sections (20 μm) of the lower thoracic cord or DRG were cut with a cryostat (Leica Microsystems, Buffalo Grove, IL, USA) and mounted on Superfrost Plus slides (Fisher Scientific, Pittsburgh, PA, USA). The slides were dried overnight and then stored at −80°C until the time of use. For fluorescent immunohistochemistry, the spinal cord and DRG sections were washed in PBS and PBS with 0.1% Triton X-100 (PBS-T), then incubated in blocking solution [5% donkey serum (#017-000-121—Jackson ImmunoResearch Laboratories, Inc., West Grove, PA, USA) in PBS-T] for 1 h at room temperature. To: (i) validate optical activation of the C-LTMRs, sections were then incubated in rabbit anti-pERK (1:200; #4370—Cell Signaling, Inc., Lake Placid, NY, USA) and/or chicken anti-green fluorescent protein (GFP; 1:500; #ab13970—Abcam, Inc., Cambridge, UK) primary antibody in blocking solution for 24 h at room temperature in a humid chamber on a gentle rotator plate (GFP antibody also detects YFP). The sections were washed in PBS-T and then incubated in Cy3-conjugated donkey anti-rabbit (1:250; #711-165-152—Jackson ImmunoResearch Laboratories, Inc., West Grove, PA, USA) and/or Cy2-conjugated donkey anti-chicken (1:100; #703-225-155—Jackson ImmunoResearch Laboratories, Inc., West Grove, PA, USA) fluorescent secondary antibody in blocking solution for 1 h at room temperature. To: (ii) reveal the expression pattern of TH projections in the spinal cord, sections were instead incubated in rabbit anti-red fluorescent protein (RFP; 1:250 in 1% BSA-PBS; #600-401-379—Rockland Immunochemicals, Inc., Gilbertsville, PA, USA) primary antibody in blocking solution for 48 h at 4°C in a humid chamber on a gentle rotator plate. The sections were washed in PBS-T and then incubated in Cy3-conjugated donkey anti-rabbit (1:500 in 1% BSA-PBS; #711-165-152—Jackson ImmunoResearch Laboratories, Inc., West Grove, PA, USA) fluorescent secondary antibody in blocking solution for 1 h at room temperature.

Following another series of washes in PBS, all sections were mounted in ProLong Gold anti-fading mounting medium (Invitrogen, Eugene, OR, USA), and coverslipped. Serial images were taken with a digital/confocal microscope (Keyence VHX-7000 Digital Light Microscope, 4× or 20× objective lens, Osaka, Japan) and stitched together to produce conglomerate images using Olympus Fluoview version 5 (Olympus America Inc., Center Valley, PA, USA).

Details of electrophysiological procedures have been published (Provost, 2019). Briefly, electrophysiological recordings were undertaken to validate optogenetic C-LTMR activation in adult SCI mice ~3 months post-injury using a TH::ChR-eYFP mouse skin-nerve preparation. Recordings were obtained from dorsal cutaneous nerves at thoracic spinal segments T8 through T12. The skin, with attached dorsal cutaneous nerves, was placed epidermal-side down over a hole in a recording dish and held in place by a cap and screws. The skin formed a seal and the dish was filled with a circulating bath maintained at 26°C. The distal end of the nerve was attached to a tight-fitting suction electrode, and the dish was placed over a circular hole on an elevated platform. The fiber optic cable was affixed to a computer-controlled robotic arm on the underside of the platform. Fiberoptic blue light pulses were used to evoke activity in TH-positive C-LTMRs. Activation was obtained from nerves following trains of optogenetic stimulation (2.5 V, 5 Hz stim., 10 5-ms pulses).

2.6 Statistical analysis and blinding

CPA and von Frey behavioral assessments were performed by the same individual. Although it was impossible to blind experimenters to the animal group due to obvious hindlimb impairment in SCI mice, adequate steps were undertaken to minimize experimental bias. For instance, RR recordings and CPA videos were coded prior to scoring. This ensured the blinding of RR data since recordings were scored independent of animal observation. Also, although CPA scoring necessitated observation of animal behavior, the simple nature of these measures (time in each chamber and crossings between chambers) left little room for experimenter bias.

Data are presented as mean ± SEM, unless otherwise noted. All behavioral (von Frey and CPA tests) and RR (resting and optically-evoked RR) data were analyzed independently for each group (sham or contusion SCI) using one-way repeated measures (RM) ANOVA with days post-operation as the within-subjects factor. Post-hoc tests corrected for multiple comparisons were performed in the case of significant results. In all cases, the choice of multiple comparisons test was selected according to GraphPad Prism (taking appropriateness for the particular dataset into account). Paired t-tests were occasionally performed as planned comparisons, as indicated in the text. Comparison between groups was accomplished using two-way RM ANOVA with days post-operation as the within-subjects factor and group (sham or SCI) as the between-subjects factor. As above, post-hoc tests corrected for multiple comparisons were performed in the case of significant results. Unpaired t-tests comparing group means were occasionally performed as planned comparisons, again as indicated in the text. To quantify the relationship between affective (CPA) pain, hindpaw mechanical sensitivity, and changes in RR, correlation analyses were undertaken at several different timepoints corresponding to observed changes in SCI mice. Statistics were performed with GraphPad Prism (GraphPad Software, La Jolla, CA, USA), with significance set at p < 0.05 and two-tailed tests.

3. Results

Prior to the two primary experiments below, mice were assigned to receive sham surgery (N = 15; eight females and seven males) or thoracic level (T10) moderate midline contusion SCI (N = 18; 10 females and eight males). One sham mouse and two SCI mice met exclusion criteria (see “2. Materials and methods” Section) and were not included in subsequent analyses. Two TH::ChR2-eYFP SCI and two naïve TH::tdTomato mice were also used for immunofluorescence. Data from male and female mice were combined as there were no statistical differences between sexes for any of the primary outcome measures assessed.

3.1 Fluorescent histology

A representative image of pERK and YFP double-labeling in DRGs following optical stimulation is shown in Figure 1A (left and top middle). Consistent with a previous report (Li et al., 2011), our fluorescent histology revealed RFP labeling indicative of TH expression in the superficial laminae of the thoracic spinal cord dorsal horn as shown in Figure 1A (bottom middle). The similarity of observed projection patterns to those previously reported suggests that we are identifying afferents whose cellular and anatomical properties are consistent with those of C-LTMRs. Selective recruitment of C-LTMRs in our study is further supported by ex-vivo electrophysiological recordings validating activation of C-LTMRs by optical stimulation in some of the same TH::ChR-eYFP mice used for behavioral experiments, shown in Figure 1A (right).

3.2 von Frey test

Sham mice did not develop any differences in sensitivity in the von Frey test from baseline to 21–28 dpo (t(9) = 1.0, p > 0.05, paired t-test). In contrast, SCI mice scored for hindpaw sensitivity at these same chronic post-injury timepoints clearly developed mechanical hypersensitivity (t(9) = 3.4, p < 0.01, paired t-test), representing a 61.4% reduction in 50% g threshold from baseline values (Figures 1B,C). SCI mice were also significantly more sensitive than their sham counterparts at the chronic timepoint (t(18) = 3.6, p < 0.005, unpaired t-test). The separate cohorts of SCI mice scored at 21 and 28 dpo presented indistinguishable mechanical thresholds by the end of testing (t(8) = 0.03, p > 0.05, unpaired t-test) with mean 50% gram thresholds of 0.74 and 0.73, respectively, so data were pooled to increase statistical power.

3.3 Conditioned place aversion (CPA)

A light-dark box CPA paradigm was used to assess affective pain responses (contextual aversion to C-LTMR stimulation by brush or laser). Chamber transitions and time spent in each chamber were monitored as depicted in Figure 2A.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Conditioned place aversion (CPA) affective pain responses following mechanical and optical C-LTMR stimulation. (A) Schematic depicting the two-chamber (light-dark) CPA apparatus, illustrating mouse positioning during stimulation (left, optical stimulation shown) and free exploration in the light chamber (middle) or transitioning between chambers (right). Adapted from Martin et al. (2022). (B) SCI mice administered mechanical brush stimulation while confined to the dark chamber later developed a preference for the light “escape” chamber that peaked 4 weeks after injury (*p < 0.05 vs. 1 dpo). (C) We found similar changes in preference following selective optical stimulation of TH-expressing sensory afferents, the C-LTMRs [*p < 0.05 SCI pre-conditioning (PreC) vs. post-conditioning (PostC); #p < 0.05 SCI vs. Sham at PostC]. For (B) and (C), horizontal dotted lines indicate 50% time spent in a non-stimulated chamber. (D,E) The total number of side-to-side transitions during CPA behavioral experiments was used as a broad assessment of locomotor activity, revealing the expected impairment after injury in SCI mice. Note that the longer duration of CPA pre- and post-stimulation sessions in the second experiment (optical stimulation) resulted in a greater overall number of side-to-side transitions compared to brush stimulation (*p < 0.05 vs. 1 dpo; #p < 0.05 SCI vs. Sham). For (B) and (D), N = 7 mice per group; for (C) and (E), N = 7 sham and N = 9 SCI.

3.3.1 Brush CPA—% time in light chamber

Overall averages combining the pre- and post-stimulation periods were used to derive our principal conclusions in regard to the aversiveness of C-LTMR-targeting brush stimulation since these values represent a more robust average (over 20-min weekly periods) of an animal’s total preference for escaping the brush-associated context. Values for % time in the light chamber of the CPA apparatus (hereafter referred to as “% in light”) at 1 dpo averaged 25.6 ± 5.0% and were statistically indistinguishable between groups (sham: 30.3 ± 7.5, SCI: 21.0 ± 6.6). One-way RM ANOVA within SCI mice revealed a significant increase in % in light over days post-surgery (F(2.0,11.8) = 5.1, p < 0.05). Comparisons between post-surgical “baselines” (at 1 dpo) and subsequent timepoints using Dunnett’s multiple comparisons tests revealed a significant increase in preference for the light chamber at all three chronic timepoints (21 dpo: 52.8 ± 8.2%, p < 0.01; 28 dpo: 55.5 ± 6.3%, p < 0.005; and 35 dpo: 50.9 ± 9.1%, p < 0.05), but not at acute or subacute timepoints (7 or 14 dpo; Figure 2B). In contrast, sham mice did not show changes in preference at any timepoint (F(1.6,9.6) = 0.42, p > 0.05, one-way RM ANOVA), and they never spent more than 42.6% of their time in the light chamber (at 7 dpo), with values even decreasing slightly at chronic timepoints (average from 21 to 35 dpo: 39.1%). Comparing the two groups using two-way RM ANOVA, there was a significant effect of days post-surgery on % in light (F(2.6,31.0) = 3.5, p < 0.05). However, there was no significant group effect (F(1,12) = 0.66, p > 0.05).

3.3.2 Optogenetic CPA—% time in non-stimulated chamber

To minimize potential ceiling effects, optogenetic experiments provided stimulation to mice in their preferred chamber (occasionally the light chamber) during conditioning sessions. Pre-conditioning values (pre-stimulation at 26 dpo) were similar to those observed in the brush CPA experiment, with both groups spending less than 30% of their time in the non-preferred chamber (sham: 25.9 ± 1.8%, SCI: 25.7 ± 5.5%). In contrast, SCI mice developed a large and significant preference for the non-stimulated chamber following three days of optical stimulation targeting C-LTMRs (51.4 ± 9.9%; t(8) = 3.4, p < 0.01, paired t-test), while sham mice showed no change in chamber preference (26.9 ± 1.4%; t(6) = 0.5, p > 0.05, paired t-test; Figure 2C). On average, SCI mice spent 90.9% more time in the non-stimulated chamber during the post-stimulation period than sham controls. Despite large intraindividual variability—especially in SCI mice—the two groups were significantly different (t(14) = 2.2, p < 0.05, unpaired t-test).

3.3.3 Transitions (brush and optogenetic CPA)

Locomotor assessment with the BMS was only undertaken at 1 dpo to confirm injury severity. However, transitions between chambers (i.e., side-to-side crosses) in the CPA paradigm were used as a broad quantification of locomotor function and development of conditioned aversion to targeted C-LTMR stimulation at later timepoints. Brush Cohort: Using two-way RM ANOVA, there was no main effect of group (SCI vs. sham) on overall transitions despite a trend (F(1,12) = 2.8, p = 0.12); however, in the same test there was a main effect of dpo (F(1.7,20.7) = 8.5, p < 0.005). There was a significant change in the number of transitions over days post-injury in SCI mice (F(1.5,9.2) = 9.8, p < 0.01, one-way RM ANOVA), with increased transitions at 14 dpo (p < 0.05) and 21–35 dpo (p < 0.005) as compared to 1 dpo using Dunnett’s multiple comparisons tests. Overall sham mouse transitions showed a trend toward varying with dpo (F(1.5,8.8) = 3.5, p = 0.09, one-way RM ANOVA). The increase in transitions from 1 dpo to chronic timepoints within SCI mice but not sham controls supported a gradual recovery of locomotor function post-injury (but not full recovery; see Figures 2D,E). Optogenetics Cohort: SCI mice performed significantly fewer transitions between the light and dark chambers than sham mice, during both the pre-stimulation test (sham: 104.7 ± 26.3, SCI: 34.8 ± 5.8; t(14) = 2.9, p < 0.05, unpaired t-test) and post-stimulation test (sham: 92.3 ± 17.5, SCI: 33.4 ± 7.0; t(14) = 3.4, p < 0.005, unpaired t-test). There was no significant change in the number of transitions from pre- to post-stimulation in either the SCI (t(8) = 0.2, p > 0.05, paired t-test) or sham (t(6) = 1.2, p > 0.05, paired t-test) mice.

3.4 Resting and evoked RR

As depicted in Figure 3A, resting RR was monitored at baseline and several post-surgical timepoints. The extended duration of optical CPA conditioning sessions also permitted recording of evoked RRs at a chronic timepoint (27–29 dpo).

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Resting and evoked RR changes in SCI mice. (A) Schematic showing respiratory monitoring with remote electric field sensors affixed to an acrylic cylindrical tube. Sensors were positioned facing a small horizontal window in the tube to allow unimpeded RR recordings in semi-restrained mice. Threshold-based detection was performed on individual records to isolate each breath and quantify RRs. (B) SCI mice receiving brush or optical stimulation CPA underwent an elevation in resting RR at 21 days following injury compared to baseline values, while RRs at acute timepoints [1 dpo and 7 dpo (subgroup not shown)] remained unchanged. N = 7 sham, N = 13 SCI (C) SCI mice average RRs (271 ± 15 breaths/min) monitored during the optical CPA paradigm were elevated overall compared to sham control average RRs (225 ± 16 breaths/min), and this was significant when data were analyzed over conditioning sessions (C1, C2, C3, two-way ANOVA, main effect of group); however, individual stimulations did not appear to increase RR (Šídák’s multiple comparisons tests, sham vs. SCI or opto. vs. control). N = 4 sham, N = 8 SCI.

3.4.1 Resting RR

There was no main effect of group (SCI vs. sham) via two-way RM ANOVA comparing all mice that underwent RR monitoring at baseline, and 1 and 21 dpo (F(1,18) = 0.16, p > 0.05). However, SCI mice showed a significant elevation in spontaneous (resting) RR over time post-injury (F(1.8,21.8) = 8.6, p < 0.005, one-way RM ANOVA). Post-hoc comparisons revealed an elevation in resting RR right at the threshold of statistical significance in SCI mice at 21 dpo compared to baseline (p = 0.05). The magnitude of the increase from baseline was substantial, as RR increased from 218 ± 9 to 258 ± 11 breaths/min on average (a 40 breaths/min increase) at this chronic timepoint (Figure 3B). In contrast, SCI mice showed no change in resting RR at 1 dpo compared to baseline (p > 0.05) or compared to sham mice (t(18) = 1.1, unpaired t-test). The subset of SCI mice tested at an acute post-injury timepoint (7 dpo) did not develop a significant increase in resting RR (t(4) = 1.6, p > 0.05, paired t-test), although the average increase from 216 to 257 breaths/min suggests this limited dataset may have been statistically underpowered.

3.4.2 Optically-evoked RR

SCI mice undergoing the 5-day CPA paradigm had a higher overall RR than sham controls over conditioning sessions 1–3 (F(1,53) = 9.1, p < 0.005, two-way RM ANOVA). However, when group averages were taken across all conditioning sessions, the difference between SCI and sham mice did not reach significance (t(10) = 1.9, p = 0.09, unpaired t-test; Figure 3C). RR was also not consistently greater immediately following optogenetic vs. control stimulation in injured mice (t(7) = 0.34, p > 0.05, paired t-test) or sham controls (t(3) = 1.2, p > 0.05, paired t-test), suggesting a dissociation between optically-evoked RR and the changes in preference observed above.

3.5 Correlations between RR increases and affective behavior after SCI

Correlation analyses were performed to assess whether changes in resting RR predicted CPA responses to brush and optical stimulation. At predetermined time points following SCI, we performed strategic comparisons to: (i) understand the specificity of RR changes for predicting aversion to mechanical brush vs. optically-induced C-LTMR activation, and (ii) relate findings with at-level stimulation to our previous study supporting acute changes in RR as predictive of below-level mechanical hypersensitivity in the von Frey test at chronic time points (Noble et al., 2019). For brush stimulation CPA, there was a significant correlation between pre-stimulation chamber preferences and baseline resting RRs in SCI mice, with higher RRs predicting a preference for the light chamber immediately after injury (r = 0.89, p < 0.05). Conversely, the same baseline RRs negatively correlated with chamber preference at 7 dpo, reflecting less aversion to the stimulated (dark) chamber following the first occurrence of mechanical truncal stimulation (r = −0.92, p < 0.05). Baseline resting RRs also negatively predicted changes in chamber preference at 28 dpo (r = −0.88, p = 0.05). In contrast, there was a negative relationship between changes in resting RR from baseline to 7 dpo and baseline CPA preference (r = −0.87, p = 0.05), and a positive relationship between 7 dpo RR changes and CPA preferences at the occasion of the first stimulation (7 dpo; r = 0.88, p = 0.05). The same changes in resting RR at 7 dpo positively predicted changes in chamber preference at 28 dpo (r = 0.96, p < 0.05; Figure 4A). In mice receiving optical stimulation CPA, resting RRs at 1 dpo negatively predicted CPA preferences for the non-stimulated chamber post-stimulation (r = −0.76, p < 0.05; Figure 4B). The same mice with higher acute RRs at 1 dpo also tended to have a clearer chamber preference at baseline, showing a tendency to avoid the aversive (non-preferred) chamber prior to stimulation (r = −0.67, p = 0.07). Resting RR at 21 dpo was strongly positively correlated with optically-evoked RR responses during CPA conditioning sessions (r = 0.88, p < 0.005), as were changes in both of these values from baseline RRs (r = 0.94, p < 0.001; Figure 4C). Across both contusion SCI groups, baseline resting RR negatively predicted changes in RR following SCI at the two post-injury timepoints assessed in both experiments, 1 dpo (r = −0.73, p < 0.005) and 21 dpo (r = −0.76, p < 0.005). That is, lower starting RRs were associated with greater RR changes experienced acutely and chronically after injury (Figure 4D), whereas changes in resting RR from baseline to 1 dpo positively correlated with changes from baseline to 21 dpo (r = 0.66, p < 0.05). These effects were also observed within the brush and optical CPA groups, when analyzed separately.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Correlations between RR changes and affective behavior. (A) Changes in resting RR at 7 dpo (a sign of acute respiratory plasticity after SCI) positively predicted changes in chamber preference at 28 dpo in the brush CPA cohort. (B) In mice receiving optical CPA, resting RRs at 1 dpo significantly predicted CPA chamber preferences post-stimulation, such that mice with higher RRs acutely after injury tended to spend a reduced percentage of their time in the CPA “escape” chamber following stimulation. (C) Changes in resting RRs at 21 dpo were bly correlated with optically-evoked RR responses during CPA conditioning sessions in this same cohort. (D) Across SCI mice in both cohorts, there was a negative correlation between baseline resting RRs and injury-induced changes in RR at 1 dpo and 21 dpo, such that mice with the lowest RRs immediately before surgery underwent the greatest RR increases after injury.

4. Discussion

This study investigated the contribution of primary afferent plasticity to neuropathic pain following SCI, focusing on C-LTMRs. We found that mechanical stimulation, and optical stimulation of TH-expressing afferents, presumably C-LTMRs, in the trunk skin induced affective pain behaviors in adult mice after SCI. We also found that SCI mice showed a significant elevation of RR at rest and during CPA sessions. All of these responses were seen at chronic timepoints, when hindpaw mechanical hypersensitivity was also evident. These results implicate C-LTMR afferent plasticity in neuropathic pain at or near the level of injury.

It has previously been shown that hyperactivity in nociceptors facilitates pain after SCI (Bedi et al., 2010; Garraway et al., 2014; Martin et al., 2019). However, whether normally non-pain transducing peripheral afferents, the C-LTMRs, can similarly drive neuropathic pain after SCI had not been shown. Expanding on a previous study (Noble et al., 2019), we now provide evidence supporting C-LTMR contributions to evoked, affective pain after SCI. These results also support the proposition that afferent plasticity is critical to the development of affective pain after SCI.

The exact mechanisms by which C-LTMRs might engage nociceptive pathways after SCI have not been elucidated in the current study, although several possibilities can be proposed. The transformation of C-LTMR responses following injury could result from changes in the intensity of C-LTMR activation and/or postsynaptic responsiveness to C-LTMR activity; for example, less C-LTMR activation or downstream responsiveness to C-LTMR activity may elicit pleasant feelings while more activity or heightened dorsal horn responsiveness after SCI may elicit pain. It is also conceivable that C-LTMRs adopt a nociceptor-like phenotype after SCI and thereby show an increase in spontaneous firing (Bedi et al., 2010), a hallmark of SCI-induced pain hypersensitivity. An alternative possibility is that C-LTMRs, like other classes of small diameter primary afferents, undergo anatomical reorganization at their central terminals (e.g., Weaver et al., 2001; Detloff et al., 2014). Central sprouting of C-LTMRs might also cause them to project to deeper dorsal horn laminae where they can in turn relay sensory information via nociceptive specific and WDR second-order neurons. Afferent sprouting, including of sympathetic fibers, has also been seen in the periphery and DRG after nerve injury (e.g., Ramer and Bisby, 1997; Ruocco et al., 2000), and this is believed to play a role in injury-induced pain (Kinnman and Levine, 1995a, b; Jänig et al., 1996; Ramer et al., 1999; Zhang et al., 2004). Afferent sprouting with concomitant increases in nociceptor receptive fields has been shown to accompany nerve growth factor-induced hyperalgesia (Pertens et al., 1999). Although the exact locus and type of C-LTMR plasticity that occurs after SCI is still unknown, we can hypothesize based on the aforementioned studies that C-LTMRs are capable of undergoing plasticity at their cell bodies (increased excitability), peripheral terminals (changes in receptive field and tuning/recruitment properties) or central terminals (afferent sprouting); and that this plasticity is critical to the expression of at-level pain hypersensitivity after SCI.

C-LTMRs have been implicated in both anti-nociceptive and nociceptive processes. Rodent models revealed a C-LTMR-specific inhibitory pathway for long-lasting analgesia, which is activated by the release of TAFA4, a chemokine-like protein found only on these afferents (Delfini et al., 2013; Kambrun et al., 2018). Slow, pleasant brushing at a velocity (3 cm/s) optimal for activation of unmyelinated CTs can also reduce heat pain (Liljencrantz et al., 2017). C-LTMRs are responsive to massage-like stroking of hairy skin in vivo and pharmacogenetic activation in freely behaving mice promotes conditioned place preference (Vrontou et al., 2013). However, C-LTMR activity has also been associated with injury-induced allodynia in both mice (Seal et al., 2009) and humans (Liljencrantz et al., 2013; Mahns and Nagi, 2013; Nagi and Mahns, 2013), and pleasant brushing can evoke allodynia in experimental models of pain including hypertonic saline infusion (Nagi et al., 2011). One potential concern in our study is the lack of an increased preference for the CPA chamber associated with C-LTMR stimulation in sham mice, given the reported pleasurable effects of activating these afferents in both mice and humans (Loken et al., 2009; Vrontou et al., 2013). However, since animals were typically stimulated in their preferred chamber (where they spent ~70%–80% of their time), it is likely that a ceiling effect precluded observation of any pleasant or rewarding effects of C-LTMR stimulation. Future studies using our model will stimulate a subpopulation of sham and SCI mice in their non-preferred chambers to assess the true polarity of C-LTMR phenotypes. Clearly, additional studies are also needed to elucidate the exact mechanisms that underlie the sufficiency of C-LTMR stimulation for eliciting pain after SCI. For instance, studies combining optogenetic and behavioral approaches with electrophysiological techniques would be valuable. Incorporation of an ex-vivo skin-nerve preparation could provide information on changes in C-LTMR recruitment properties and receptive field maps, while an isolated DRG neuron preparation could be used to investigate the emergence of spontaneous activity in C-LTMRs (Bedi et al., 2010), after SCI.

Another key observation noted in this study is the relationship between changes in resting RR and affective pain behavior in SCI subjects. Resting RR was borderline increased at 21 dpo (p = 0.05), a finding that paralleled effects observed in adult rats at earlier timepoints (Teng et al., 1999, 2003; Noble et al., 2019). We also found that RR was acutely increased during the same CPA paradigm that successfully evoked a pain response, but this effect occurred throughout the test rather than being restricted to the optical stimulation period. Respiratory complications frequently accompany SCI in humans (Jackson and Groomes, 1994). Because sensory and autonomic systems interact, high RR may be a proxy for the deleterious effects of sympathetic overactivation on maladaptive pain after SCI. This notion is supported by previous studies showing that nociceptive stimuli activate the sympathetic nervous system, thereby resulting in increased heart rate and RR (Wolf and Hardy, 1941; Culman et al., 1997; Loggia et al., 2011; Santuzzi et al., 2013). Building on these reports, we now show that after lower thoracic SCI, touch-transducing primary afferents promote pain hypersensitivity and may concurrently increase RRs. Surprisingly, correlation analyses revealed that while changes in resting RR predicted aversion to at-level brush stimulation after SCI, this same effect did not hold true for C-LTMR-specific optogenetic stimulation. Therefore, it appears that complex relations between RRs and affective pain after SCI are not mediated exclusively by C-LTMRs, but instead may involve multiple cutaneous afferent subpopulations, and/or central circuits. The most likely possibility is that, while C-LTMRs contribute to affective pain after SCI, the mechanisms linking SCI pain to RR changes are largely C-LTMR-independent or depend on a multitude of factors. This is supported by a previous study, which demonstrated a correlation between RRs and paw withdrawal thresholds (i.e., C-LTMR-independent pain) in the von Frey test (Noble et al., 2019).

After peripheral nerve injury or inflammation, sympathetic stimulation excites nociceptors (Habler et al., 1987; Hu and Zhu, 1989; Sato and Perl, 1991; Sato et al., 1993), and norepinephrine can activate nociceptors after injury (Hu et al., 2000; Tanimoto et al., 2011). Our observed aversive effects of optogenetic stimulation after SCI could plausibly be produced by the activation of sympathetic postganglionic neurons that excite nociceptors already sensitized by SCI, with the resulting nociceptor (rather than C-LTMR) activity promoting CPA behavior. While our study design cannot rule out this indirect mechanism, immunohistochemistry in SCI mice provided evidence of direct activation of C-LTMRs via optical stimulation (i.e., TH-positive DRG neurons ipsilateral to the dermatomal field of optical stimulation were selectively recruited; Figure 1A, left and top middle). This supports the specificity of our stimulation paradigm for C-LTMRs, as does their known molecular composition and spinal innervation pattern (Li et al., 2011; Figure 1A, bottom middle). Sympathetic postganglionic axons may sprout in the DRG following peripheral nerve injury to enhance sensory neuron activation or even underlie synchronized cluster firing; however, this functional coupling appears to be diffusion-based rather than via direct connections with soma membrane (Chung et al., 1996; Shinder et al., 1999; Zheng et al., 2022). Future studies should clarify the relationship between primary afferents and sympathetic efferents as well as their relative necessity or sufficiency for initiating autonomic motor and behavioral responses after SCI. Nevertheless, we acknowledge that there is a need for caution in interpreting our results until future mechanistic studies are performed to definitively investigate a sympathetic contribution.

The present study introduces a novel approach to assess at-level SCI pain. Although at- and below-level pain is reported by patients with SCI, finding appropriate research models to assess at-level, non-reflexive pain has been challenging. In fact, very few laboratory methodologies have been implemented (Christensen and Hulsebosch, 1997). In addition to demonstrating that RR is a measurable index of at-level pain (Noble et al., 2019), here, we designed a minimally stressful, non-invasive tool to show that truncal stimulation, at dermatomes surrounding the lesion, induced a CPA response. This strategy was especially important since it enabled us to assess C-LTMRs based on their presence in hairy, but not glabrous, skin (Lou et al., 2013). In addition, we show for the first time that supraspinal, non-reflexive pain responses are evoked by stimulation of C-LTMRs following SCI, at a time when mice also display hindpaw mechanical hypersensitivity. Together, these findings confirm the expression of at- and below-level pain after SCI (Finnerup et al., 2001; Siddall et al., 2003; Felix et al., 2007). Mechanistically, dorsal horn damage after injury could lead to deficits in C-LTMR sensory processing and impair input to supraspinal brain centers, including those involved in the modulation of pain affect. C-LTMR dysregulation following dorsal horn injury may also increase pain aversion via facilitated afferent activation, resulting in reduced C-LTMR-mediated hedonic touch processing and a loss of normal analgesic function (Liljencrantz and Olausson, 2014). Plasticity in pain affect and RR responses after SCI could have different underlying pathways (e.g., see Teng et al., 1999, 2003).

A notable limitation of our study is the observed difference in the magnitude of conditioned aversion evoked by optical vs. mechanical C-LTMR stimulation. Several explanations based on our experimental design might account for the difference. First, the increased frequency of testing in mice receiving brush stimulation (once per week vs. over one 5-day period) could account for the weaker effects observed with this stimulus. Such an occurrence could constitute either habituation to repeated exposure to a stimulus, or alternatively sensitization to the experimental context. The latter possibility could explain the greater behavioral aversion that developed in sham control mice receiving brush vs. those receiving optical stimulation, potentially obscuring group differences for the brush. Second, it is likely that the reduced length of pre- and post-stimulation sessions in mechanical brush experiments (10 min) vs. later optogenetic experiments tailored to the timeline of brush outcomes (30 min) was a key contributing factor. Brush stimulation, despite being tuned to activate C-LTMRs, will undoubtedly activate other sensory afferent subpopulations as well and is thus non-selective. Therefore, a third possibility is that broadly-administered brush stimulation might mask the selective effects of C-LTMR stimulation by activating cutaneous afferent subpopulations that oppose or neutralize the impact of C-LTMRs. Using a CPA paradigm similar to ours, Chamessian et al. recently found no impact of cutaneous Aβ-LTMR stimulation on aversive behavior (Chamessian et al., 2019).

The present study demonstrates that cutaneous mechanoreceptors can drive pain behavior after SCI. While there is previous evidence suggesting that non-nociceptor Aβ afferents can undergo a phenotypic switch to contribute to inflammatory hypersensitivity (Neumann et al., 1996), a similar change in the properties of mechanosensitive non-nociceptors in chronic SCI pain has not yet been shown. Although we have established the sufficiency of C-LTMR stimulation for inducing an aversive CPA response, future studies should optically inhibit C-LTMRs to establish necessity (e.g., in TH-crossed, archaerhodopsin-expressing mice), and thereby fully assess causality. Studies should also investigate whether pain-associated neural responses are attenuated in these transgenic models.

Our results strongly support recent studies demonstrating the transformation of C-LTMRs into pain-transmitting afferents after injury (Seal et al., 2009; Mahns and Nagi, 2013; Liljencrantz and Olausson, 2014). In addition to this crucial observation, we provide the first experimental evidence that C-LTMRs contribute to evoked, affective pain after SCI. By utilizing a novel and feasible behavioral paradigm, in combination with optogenetic technology, we show that C-LTMR activation is sufficient for at-level neuropathic pain after SCI. Moreover, we report a complex interaction between C-LTMR stimulation and breathing, assessed through RRs. Like affective pain, RRs were elevated post-injury. Counteractive manipulations such as slowing respiration could provide a voluntary portal to autonomic nervous system control (Noble and Hochman, 2019) to reduce pain hypersensitivity, potentially

留言 (0)

沒有登入
gif