High bladder pressure reduces the ability to filter out interference from distractors in low perceptual load condition

Allport, D. A. (1977). On knowing the meaning of words we are unable to report: The effects of visual masking. In Dornic, S. (Ed.), Attention and performance VI (pp. 505–533). Erlbaum.
Google Scholar Anderson, M. C., Green, C. (2001). Suppressing unwanted memories by executive control. Nature, 410(6826), 366–369.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline | ISI Anil, A. E., Kivircik, B. B., Batur, S., Kabakçi, E., Kitis, A., Güven, E., . . .Arkar, H. (2003). The Turkish version of the Auditory Consonant Trigram Test as a measure of working memory: A normative study. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 17(2), 159–169.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Aron, A. R., Fletcher, P. C., Bullmore, E. T., Sahakian, B. J., Robbins, T. W. (2003). Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans. Nature Neuroscience, 6(2), 115–116.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Aron, A. R., Robbins, T. W., Poldrack, R. A. (2004). Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(4), 170–177.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline | ISI Bannon, S., Gonsalvez, C. J., Croft, R. J., Boyce, P. M. (2002). Response inhibition deficits in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Research, 110(2), 165–174.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline | ISI Benoni, H., Tsal, Y. (2010). Where have we gone wrong? Perceptual load does not affect selective attention. Vision Research, 50(13), 1292–1298.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Berkman, E. T., Burklund, L., Lieberman, M. D. (2009). Inhibitory spillover: Intentional motor inhibition produces incidental limbic inhibition via right inferior frontal cortex. NeuroImage, 47(2), 705–712.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Boot, W. R., Brockmole, J. R., Simons, D. J. (2005). Attention capture is modulated in dual-task situations. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(4), 662–668.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Brand-D’Abrescia, M., Lavie, N. (2008). Task coordination between and within sensory modalities: Effects on distraction. Perception & Psychophysics, 70(3), 508–515.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Perception and communication. Pergamon Press.
Google Scholar | Crossref Broadbent, D. E., Cooper, P. F., FitzGerald, P., Parkes, K. R. (1982). The cognitive failures questionnaire (CFQ) and its correlates. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 21(1), 1–16.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline | ISI Brown, J. (1958). Some tests of the decay theory of immediate memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 10(1), 12–21.
Google Scholar | SAGE Journals | ISI Catarino, A., Küpper, C. S., Werner-Seidler, A., Dalgleish, T., Anderson, M. C. (2015). Failing to forget: Inhibitory-control deficits compromise memory suppression in posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychological Science, 26(5), 604–616.
Google Scholar | SAGE Journals | ISI Chamberlain, S. R., Fineberg, N. A., Blackwell, A. D., Robbins, T. W., Sahakian, B. J. (2006). Motor inhibition and cognitive flexibility in obsessive-compulsive disorder and trichotillomania. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163(7), 1282–1284.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Chen, Z., Cave, K. R. (2013). Perceptual load vs. dilution: The roles of attentional focus, stimulus category, and target predictability. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, Article 327.
Google Scholar | Crossref Chen, Z., Cave, K. R. (2015). Singleton search is guided by knowledge of the target, but maybe it shouldn’t be. Vision Research, 115, 92–103.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline de Fockert, J. W. (2013). Beyond perceptual load and dilution: A review of the role of working memory in selective attention. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, Article 287.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline de Fockert, J. W., Bremner, A. J. (2011). Release of inattentional blindness by high working memory load: Elucidating the relationship between working memory and selective attention. Cognition, 121(3), 400–408.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline de Fockert, J. W., Rees, G., Frith, C. D., Lavie, N. (2001). The role of working memory in visual selective attention. Science, 291(5509), 1803–1806.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline | ISI Deutsch, J. A., Deutsch, D. (1963). Attention: Some theoretical considerations. Psychological Review, 70(1), 80–90.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline | ISI Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135–168.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline | ISI Driver, J. (2001). A selective review of selective attention research from the past century. British Journal of Psychology, 92(1), 53–78.
Google Scholar | Crossref Eriksen, B. A., Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16(1), 143–149.
Google Scholar | Crossref Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline | ISI Fenn, E., Blandón-Gitlin, I., Coons, J., Pineda, C., Echon, R. (2015). The inhibitory spillover effect: Controlling the bladder makes better liars. Consciousness and Cognition, 37, 112–122.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Forster, S., Lavie, N. (2007). High perceptual load makes everybody equal. Psychological Science, 18(5), 377–381.
Google Scholar | SAGE Journals | ISI Frings, C., Schneider, K. K., Fox, E. (2015). The negative priming paradigm: An update and implications for selective attention. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(6), 1577–1597.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Gaspelin, N., Luck, S. J. (2018). The role of inhibition in avoiding distraction by salient stimuli. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(1), 79–92.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Geng, J. J. (2014). Attentional mechanisms of distractor suppression. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23(2), 147–153.
Google Scholar | SAGE Journals | ISI Gil-Gómez de Liaño, B., Stablum, F., Umiltà, C. (2016). Can concurrent memory load reduce distraction? A replication study and beyond. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(1), e1–e12.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Griffiths, D., Tadic, S. D. (2008). Bladder control, urgency, and urge incontinence: Evidence from functional brain imaging. Neurourology and Urodynamics, 27(6), 466–474.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Itthipuripat, S., Cha, K., Byers, A., Serences, J. T. (2017). Two different mechanisms support selective attention at different phases of training. PLoS Biology, 15(6), Article e2001724.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Johnston, W. A., Dark, V. J. (1982). In defense of intraperceptual theories of attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8(3), 407–421.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Jonides, J., Smith, E. E., Marshuetz, C., Koeppe, R. A., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (1998). Inhibition in verbal working memory revealed by brain activation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 95(14), 8410–8413.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Karakaş, S., Erdoğan, E., Sak, L., Soysal, A. Ş., Ulusoy, T., Ulusoy, İ. Y., Alkan, S. (1999). Stroop Test TBAG Form: Standardisation for Turkish Culture, Reliability and Validity. Turkish Journal of Clinical Psyhiatry. 2(2), 75–88
Google Scholar Konstantinou, N., Beal, E., King, J. R., Lavie, N. (2014). Working memory load and distraction: Dissociable effects of visual maintenance and cognitive control. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76(7), 1985–1997.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Lappin, J. S., Eriksen, C. W. (1966). Use of a delayed signal to stop a visual reaction-time response. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72(6), 805–811.
Google Scholar | Crossref Lavie, N. (1995). Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(3), 451–468.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline | ISI Lavie, N. (2000). Selective attention and cognitive control: Dissociating attentional functions through different types of load. In Monsell, S., Driver, J. (Eds.), Attention and performance XVIII (pp. 175–194). MIT Press.
Google Scholar Lavie, N. (2001). Capacity limits in selective attention: Behavioral evidence and implications for neural activity. In Braun, J., Koch, C., Davis, J. L. (Eds.), Visual attention and cortical circuits (pp. 49–68). MIT Press.
Google Scholar Lavie, N. (2005). Distracted and confused? Selective attention under load. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(2), 75–82.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline | ISI Lavie, N. (2010). Attention, distraction, and cognitive control under load. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(3), 143–148.
Google Scholar | SAGE Journals | ISI Lavie, N., Cox, S. (1997). On the efficiency of visual selective attention: Efficient visual search leads to inefficient distractor rejection. Psychological Science, 8(5), 395–396.
Google Scholar | SAGE Journals | ISI Lavie, N., de Fockert, J. (2006). Frontal control of attentional capture in visual search. Visual Cognition, 14(4–8), 863–876.
Google Scholar | Crossref Lavie, N., Hirst, A., De Fockert, J. W., Viding, E. (2004). Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(3), 339–354.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline | ISI Lavie, N., Torralbo, A. (2010). Dilution: A theoretical burden or just load? A reply to Tsal and Benoni (2010). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(6), 1657–1664.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Lavie, N., Tsal, Y. (1994). Perceptual load as a major determinant of the locus of selection in visual attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 56(2), 183–197.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Mayr, S., Buchner, A. (2007). Negative priming as a memory phenomenon. Zeitschrift für Psychologie/Journal of Psychology, 215(1), 35–51.
Google Scholar | Crossref | ISI Menon, V., Adleman, N. E., White, C. D., Glover, G. H., Reiss, A. L. (2001). Error-related brain activation during a Go/NoGo response inhibition task. Human Brain Mapping, 12(3), 131–143.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline | ISI Miller, J. (1987). Evidence of preliminary response preparation from a divided attention task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13(3), 425–434.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100.

留言 (0)

沒有登入
gif