Neonatal procedural pain affects state, but not trait anxiety behavior in adult rats

1 INTRODUCTION

Painful and invasive procedures are common for neonates hospitalized in the neonatal intensive care unit (NICU), with up to 14 painful procedures experienced each day (Cruz et al., 2016). Early exposure to painful procedures has been shown to contribute to long-term negative consequences including alterations in somatosensation, anxiety, and depression (Grunau et al., 2001; Peters et al., 2005; Ranger et al., 2014; Walker et al., 2018; Williams & Lascelles, 2020). Indeed, early exposure to painful procedures affects neurodevelopment, leading to a reduction in amygdala and thalamus volumes, which consequently can impair affective functioning (Brummelte et al., 2012; Chau et al., 2019; Cismaru et al., 2016; Duerden et al., 2018; Walker et al., 2018). Interestingly, survivors of neonatal repetitive pain show higher rates of internalizing behaviors, and higher anxiety and pain catastrophizing in childhood, infancy and early adulthood (Botting et al., 1997; Hayes & Sharif, 2009; Johnson et al., 2010; Ranger et al., 2014; Walker et al., 2018). Although clinical evidence shows that affective behavior in infants and children is altered by early-life pain exposure, clinical data on the impact of early life pain on anxiety in adulthood remain limited.

Preclinical evidence suggests that anxiety in adolescence and adulthood is affected by neonatal procedural pain, but findings are heterogeneous (Anand et al., 1999; Chen et al., 2016; Page et al., 2005; Schellinck et al., 2003; Zuke et al., 2019). In more detail, neonatal repetitive procedural pain has been shown to increase (Davis et al., 2018; Schellinck et al., 2003), decrease (Anand et al., 1999; Chen et al., 2016; Page et al., 2005; Zuke et al., 2019), or fail to show an effect on adult anxiety behavior in rodents (Anand et al., 1999; Mooney-Leber & Brummelte, 2020; Ranger et al., 2018). In both humans and laboratory animals, anxiety is not a unitary phenomenon and includes both innate (trait) and situation-evoked (state) anxiety. Trait anxiety is an enduring feature that is consistent across time and context, whereas state anxiety is anxiety experienced at one particular time and can be contextual and conditioned (Ohl, 2003). Behavioral paradigms designed to measure trait and state anxiety have been developed in rodents. The open field test (OFT) represents normal or trait anxiety, as rodent behavior in this test reflects a natural balance between exploratory and escaping drives (Hawley et al., 2011; Prut & Belzung, 2003). On the other hand, the elevated zero maze (EZM) models situation evoked (state) anxiety that is contextual to the anxiogenic stimuli present (including openessness and elevation). Previous studies on the long-term effect of neonatal pain used the term “anxiety” without an a priori assumption of whether or not trait or state anxiety was represented. The type of behavioral essay used, and thus the type of anxiety (trait vs. state) measured, might explain some of the contradictory findings as most often state but not trait anxiety seems affected by neonatal procedural pain. The aim of the present study is to investigate the long-term effects of repetitive neonatal procedural pain on trait and state anxiety behavior in adulthood, in a well-established animal model of repetitive needle pricks (NPs) (Knaepen et al., 2013; van den Hoogen et al., 2020). Trait anxiety is measured in the OFT, whereas state anxiety is measured in the EZM.

2 METHODS 2.1 Animals

Sprague–Dawley males and females were purchased from Charles River Laboratory and were mated at Maastricht University to produce eight litters. Dams were transferred to the experimental room at gestational day (G) 16, and all pups were born on G21. Litters were culled to a maximum of N = 10 to ensure equal caretaking by the dam. Each experimental litter included a balanced representation of neonatal condition, and equal distribution by sex per group (Table 1). A maximum of one or two male and/or female pups were taken from each litter for each condition to control for any litter effects (Chapman & Stern, 1979). Pups were weaned at postnatal day (P)21 and randomly housed in same-sex groups of two or three in individually ventilated cages, in temperature (19–24°C) and humidity (55 ± 15%) controlled rooms with a reversed 12:12-h day-night cycle (lights on 7 p.m.–7 a.m.). Ad libitum water and food was available for the duration of the study.

TABLE 1. Distribution of sex and condition in experimental litters NP TC UC Litter f m f m f m 1 1 2 2 1 2 2 1 1 2 3 2 2 4 2 2 0 1 5 2 2 6 2 2 2 2 7 1 2 2 1 8 3 5 Abbreviations: f, female; m, male; NP, needle prick; TC, tactile control; UC, undisturbed control.

All experiments were performed in accordance with the European Directive for the Protection of Vertebrate Animal Use for Experimental and Other Scientific Purposes (2010/63/EU) and were approved by the Committee for Experiments on Animals, Maastricht, The Netherlands (DEC 2017-017).

2.2 Neonatal procedures and mechanical sensitivity

A neonatal repetitive NP model was used as previously described (Knaepen et al., 2013; van den Hoogen et al., 2020). All pups were randomly assigned to neonatal conditions at birth (using a computer-generated randomization list). Pups received either noxious NPs (n = 9 males, n = 8 females) or tactile stimulations (tactile control (TC); n = 7 males, n = 7 females) in the midplantar surface of the left hind paw four times a day (at 08:00, 09:00, 10:00, and 11:00 a.m.), from day of birth (postnatal day (P0) to P7. For each procedure, the nest was briefly separated from the dam. Paw withdrawal thresholds (PWT) of the ipsilateral and contralateral hind paws were assessed before (baseline, BL) and 1, 3, and 5 h after the last noxious or tactile stimulation using a dorsal von Frey design (Marsh et al., 1999). Ascending Von Frey filaments (bending force 0.407, 0.692, 1.202, 2.041, 3.63 (from P4 onwards) and 5.495 (from P6 onwards)) were applied five times to the dorsal surface of the hind paws. The number of positive responses (withdrawal or flinching behavior evoked by the filaments) were recorded, and behavioral testing was discontinued when five positive responses were observed. A 50% PWT was calculated using sigmoidal curve fitting in GraphPad Prism 8 (GraphPad Software, San Diego, CA, USA). Separate nests were left undisturbed during the first postnatal week (undisturbed control (UC); n = 8 males, n = 8 females).

2.3 Assessment of mechanical sensitivity throughout development

Mechanical sensitivity of the hind paws was assessed weekly from weaning to adulthood (3–8 weeks of age; P21–P56). Animals were placed in individual Plexiglas cages on an elevated mesh floor and were allowed to acclimatize to the behavioral set-up before testing. Von Frey filaments with logarithmically increasing stiffness (bending forces 1.202, 2.041, 3.63, 5.495, 8.511, 15.136 and 28.84 g; Stoelting, USA) were applied to the mid-plantar surface of the hind paws for 5 s. Mechanical sensitivity was assessed using the up-down method, and the mechanical force resulting in a 50% withdrawal frequency was assigned as the PWT (Chaplan et al., 1994).

2.4 Adult anxiety behavior testing

Anxiety-like behavior was assessed in adulthood (8 weeks of age) under dim light conditions (2 lux, 0.02 W/m3 infrared light). Animals were allowed to acclimatize to the experimental room before testing. To control for possible carry-over effects from the OFT to the EZM, the animals were allowed a recovery period of 3 days. The animal order of testing was randomized, and researchers were blinded to conditions during testing.

2.5 Trait anxiety in the OFT

To assess trait anxiety, animals were introduced into the center of an open arena (100 × 100 × 40 cm) and allowed to freely explore for 5 min. In-between each session, the arena was thoroughly cleaned with 70% ethanol. Behavior of the rats was automatically tracked and analyzed using a video tracking system (Ethovision Pro, Noldus, the Netherlands). While in the arena, time spend in the center (%), center crossings (number of entries from and to the center), and total distance travelled (locomotor path in whole arena in cm) were recorded and analyzed using Ethovision XT10 (Noldus).

2.6 State anxiety in the EZM

Adult animals were exposed to the EZM (Shepherd et al., 1994) to assess state anxiety. The EZM is an elevated black annular arena (100 cm diameter, 10 cm path width and 70 cm above floor level) divided into four equal quadrants. Two opposite quadrants are “closed” by black Perspex walls (closed arms), while the remaining two quadrants are “open” (open arms). Animals were placed in the middle of an open arm facing one of the closed arms and allowed to explore the maze for 5 min. Between each session, the arena was thoroughly cleaned with 70% ethanol. Movement of the rats was recorded and scored using an infrared video camera connected to a video tracking system (Ethovision Pro, Noldus). Behavioral measures recorded included: time spend in open and closed arms, latency to enter each area, and open arm entries. Anxiety-like behavior was classified as the time in the open arms as a percentage of total (corrected) trial time (= total trial duration – latency to enter first closed arm). In addition, head dips (paws in closed arm, head stretched forward into open arm) were hand scored by an experimenter, blinded to the animal's condition.

2.7 Statistical analysis

All data is presented as mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM). For the neonatal period, an area under the curve (AUC) analysis was performed. The AUC was calculated based on the PWTs for each group over the whole neonatal period (P0–7) and TC and NP animals were compared with an unpaired t-test. Differences in mechanical sensitivity between TC, NP, and UC throughout development were analyzed using a repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) with Tukey post-hoc correction (to control for multiple testing). As litters can affect anxiety-like behavioral measures, litter was used as unit of analysis, averaging data from pups of the same condition per sex for each litter (Chapman & Stern, 1979; Lazic, 2012). An additional analysis was performed using pups as unit of analysis (see supplemental data). A two-way ANOVA was performed to compare the effects of condition and sex on anxiety-like behavioral measurements. If sex effects were not observed, data were pooled by condition to increase power. All statistical analysis were conducted using Graphpad Prism 8.0 (GraphPad Software) and results were considered significant at p < 0.05.

3 RESULTS 3.1 Mechanical sensitivity throughout development

NP animals showed a significantly decreased AUC for ipsilateral PWTs during the entire neonatal period compared with TC animals, indicating the development of acute mechanical hypersensitivity in pups who received repetitive needle pricks (t (29) = 8.038; p < 0.001; Figure 1(a)). During development from weaning (P21) to adulthood (P56), ipsilateral PWT significantly increased over time (F (5,220) = 52.66; p < 0.0001), but did not differ between neonatal conditions (F(2,44) = 0.254; p = 0.777); Figure 1(b)). Contralateral PWTs significantly increased over time (F(5, 220) = 52.58; p < 0.01), but were not significantly different between conditions at any time point during the neonatal phase (t(29) = 0.8311; p = 0.4127) or development (F(2,44) = 0.2538; p = 0.777; Figure 2). No effect of sex on mechanical sensitivity was observed (p > 0.05).

image

Mechanical sensitivity (a and b) and anxiety behavior (c and d) after repetitive neonatal needle pricking. (a) Repetitive neonatal needle pricks (NP; n = 17) result in a decreased paw-withdrawal threshold (PWT) in the ipsilateral paw as compared with the repetitive tactile stimulation (TC; n = 14), shown by a lower area under the curve (AUC) over the whole neonatal period for NP animals compared with the TC animals (t(29) = 8.038; p < 0.001). (b) Ipsilateral PWTs increase over time for all groups (F(2,220) = 52.66; p < 0.01), did not significantly differ between NP, TC, and undisturbed control (UC; n = 16) animals (F(2,44) = 0.254; p = 0.777). (c) The percentage of time spend in the anxio-genic (center) region of the open field test (OFT) in 8-week-old animals did not significantly differ between neonatal conditions (F(2,19) = 0.1206; p = 0.8871) or sex (F(1,19) = 0.2284; p = 0.6382). (d) The percentage of time spend in the anxio-genic (open arms) region of the elevated zero maze (EZM) differs significantly between neonatal conditions (F(2,19) = 3.966; p = 0.0364) but not sex (F(1,19) = 0.0001; p = 0.9997). NP animals spend more time in the open arms of the elevated zero maze as compared with the UC animals (NP 35.21 ± 2.47; UD 24.63 ± 1.02; p = 0.0286). Data are presented as mean ± SEM, *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01

image

Contralateral mechanical sensitivity during the neonatal week and throughout development. (a) Area under the curve analysis (AUC) over the whole neonatal period does not show significant differences in contralateral paw-withdrawal thresholds (PWT) between repetitive needle pricking (NP; n = 17) and tactile control animals (TCs; n = 14) (t(29) = 0.831; p = 0.4127). (b) Contralateral PWTs increase over time for all groups (F(5,220) = 52,58; p < 0.01) but was not significantly different between conditions at any time point (F(2,44) = 0.2538; p = 0.777). Data presented as mean ± SEM, *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01

3.2 Adult anxiety behavior

Trait anxiety as assessed in the OFT in adulthood showed no significant effect of condition (F(2, 19) = 0.3218; p = 0.7287; Figure 1(c)) or sex (F(1,19) = 0.2284; p = 0.6382; Figure 1(c)) on percentage of time spend in the center of the arena. The frequency of center crossings in the OFT showed a significant interaction effect for sex × condition (F(2,19) = 4.194; p = 0.0310; Figure 3), with females showing a trend to more center crossings in the TC group only (p = 0.060). Locomotor activity was affected by sex (F(1,19) = 6.701; p = 0.0180) but not condition (F(2,19) = 0.8271; p = 0.4625), with females showing higher locomotor activity as compared with the males (Figure 3).

image

Exploratory behavior (head dips, open arm entries, and center crossings) and locomotor behavior of adult rats exposed to repetitive needle pricking, tactile stimulation, or left undisturbed as neonates. Females show significantly more exploratory behavior as compared with the males, measured by the frequency of head dips (a; F(1,19) = 8.597; p < 0.01) and open arm entries (b; F(1,19) = 4.683; p = 0.0434). (c) A significant interaction effect is observed in the frequency of center crossings (F(2,19) = 4,194; p = 0.0310), with females tending to cross the center more frequently as compared with the males in the TC group only (p = 0.06). (d) Locomotor activity in the open field test (OFT) was affected by sex (F(1,19) = 6.701; p = 0.0180) but not condition (F(2,19) = 0.8271; p = 0.4525), with females show significantly higher total distance travelled as compared with the males. NP, needle prick animals; TC, tactile controls; UC, undisturbed controls. Data presented as mean ± SEM, *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; # sign. effect of males versus females

State anxiety as assessed in EZM demonstrates that the percentage of time spend in the open arms significantly differed between groups (F(2,19) = 3.966; p = 0.0364; Figure 1(d)) but was unaffected by sex (F(1,19) = 0.0001; p = 0.9997). When pooling data by condition, the significant difference between neonatal conditions in the time in open arms of the EZM persisted (F(2,22) = 3.777; p = 0.0389). Posthoc analysis revealed that 8-week-old NP animals spend significantly more time in the open arms of the EZM, compared with the UC (NP 35.21 ± 2.47; UC 24.63 ± 1.02; p = 0.0303; Figure 1(d)). Neonatal condition did not influence exploratory behavior (head dips and open arm entries) in the EZM (p > 0.05). However, females showed significantly more exploratory behavior, measured by the number of head dips (F(1,19) = 8.597; p < 0.01) and open arm entries (F(1,19) = 4.683; p = 0.0434; Figure 3). Using pups as unit of analysis (i.e., not averaging data of pups from the same condition per sex for each litter) did not influence behavioral outcome in the OFT and EZM (see Figure S1).

4 DISCUSSION

This study investigates the long-term effect of neonatal procedural pain on adult state and trait anxiety behavior in a well-established animal model of neonatal repetitive needle pricking. Our data show that a combination of repetitive neonatal procedural pain and handling decreases state, but not trait anxiety behavior in adult rats. At the same time, neonatal needle pricking results in acute mechanical hypersensitivity but does not produce long-term changes in baseline mechanical sensitivity.

Consistent with previous studies, limited numbers of needle pricks from P0 to P7 leads to robust acute hypersensitivity without altering mechanical sensitivity throughout development (Knaepen et al., 2012; Knaepen et al., 2013; Ranger et al., 2018; van den Hoogen et al., 2020; Van den Hoogen et al., 2016). However, more widespread (all paws), prolonged (14 vs. 7 days), or more intense (paw incision or 10 needle pricks per day) tissue breaking procedures lead to altered baseline sensitivity (Carmo et al., 2016; Chen et al., 2016; Nuseir et al., 2017).

Whether neonatal procedural pain (“first-hit”) affects anxiety in later life has been topic of debate. Previously, neonatal repetitive procedural pain has been shown to increase (Davis et al., 2018; Schellinck et al., 2003), decrease (Anand et al., 1999; Chen et al., 2016; Page et al., 2005; Zuke et al., 2019) or fail to show an effect on adult anxiety behavior in rodents (Anand et al., 1999; Mooney-Leber & Brummelte, 2020; Ranger et al., 2018). The effect of neonatal repetitive procedural pain on anxiety seems to be dependent on the behavioral essay used, pointing at a distinct profile of anxiety (trait or state) affected. Rodent anxiety behavior in the OFT reflects the natural balance between exploratory and escaping drives, and therefore more likely represents inherent or trait anxiety (Hawley et al., 2011; Prut & Belzung, 2003). Our study shows that four needle pricks a day between P0 and P7 as a model of procedural neonatal pain does not influence trait anxiety, including time in the center of the arena and number of center crossings in the OFT, consistent with previous studies (Mooney-Leber & Brummelte, 2020; Ranger et al., 2018). The open-field entries from PVC tubes, or the time in the center of a circular arena was also unaffected by neonatal procedural pain (Anand et al., 1999; Low & Fitzgerald, 2012), suggesting that neonatal procedural pain does not influence trait anxiety in adulthood. Reduced exploratory behavior might confound measures of anxiety, as they depend on general locomotor activity (i.e., movement) (Ohl, 2003). However, locomotor activity was unaffected by neonatal condition in our study and did therefore not influence overall exploratory behavior and thus state anxiety in the OFT.

In contrast to the OFT, the EZM confronts the animal with an anxiety-provoking situation such as height and avoidance of open spaces, modeling state-induced anxiety (Goes et al., 2009). Adult animals exposed to a combination of repetitive neonatal needle pricking and handling showed lower state anxiety levels, as they spend more time in the open arms of the EZM as compared with their UCs in this study. Previous behavioral testing paradigms and handling can influence behavior in the EZM (Bailey & Crawley, 2009). As all animals were exposed to similar testing protocols and handling, this effect should be similar across all groups. Previous studies using the same neonatal procedural pain model have also reported decreased state anxiety in the elevated plus maze (EPM) as compared with the touched controls or irregularly stimulated animals (Page et al., 2005; Zuke et al., 2019). One study that increased the number of needle pricks per day did not show a long-term effect on anxiety in the EPM, compared with the touched or handled controls (Ranger et al., 2018). In addition, repetitive needle pricking led to an increase in anxiety behavior when tested during adolescence (Schellinck et al., 2003). Therefore, inconsistencies in the manipulation procedure (4 vs. 10 NP daily), the developmental stage during testing (adolescent vs. adult), control groups used (touch, handling or undisturbed) as well as differences in behavioral essay used might account for the different outcomes (Table 2). Neonatal procedural pain has also been shown to reduce later-life fear conditioning (Davis et al., 2018), suggesting that our findings are most consistent with a reduction in state anxiety in adult rodents.

TABLE 2. Summary of literature on the effect of neonatal procedural pain on adult anxiety Model Controls Species Time of modulation Time of testing Behavioral test Outcomea References RNP (4 NPs in left hind paw)

TC, UC

Rats P0–P7 Adult (P56) OFT, EZM

↑ time in open arms

No effect on open arm entries, head dips, time in center and center crossings

This study RNP (4 NPs balanced over all paws) TC, UC Rats P0–P7 Adult (P100) OFT, EPM

No effect on time in center or center entries

↑ time in open arms

Page et al. (2005) RNP (10 NPs balanced over all paws) TC, handling Mice P1–P6 Adult (P60) OFT, EPM No effect on time spend in center or time in open arms Ranger et al. (2014) RNP (4 NPs in left hind paw) TC, UC Rats P1–P7 P24–P25 EPM

↑ time in open arms

No effect on (open) arm entries

Zuke et al. (2019) RNP (2 NPs on front and hind paw) Sham Mice P8–P14 P30 EPM

↓ open arm entries

↓ time in open arms

↓ head dips

↑ stretch attend postures

Schellinck et al. (2003) RNP (4 NPs balanced over all paws) TC Rats P0–P7 P24, P87 OFT

↑ distance in center at P24 and P86

↑ center entries at P24

↑ time in center at P24 and P87

↑ total entries at P87

Chen et al. (2016) RNP (8 NPs on P1, 6 NPs on P2, 4 NPs on P3, 2 pokes on P4, balanced over all paws) TC, touch & isolation, UC Rats P1–P4 Adult (P80) OFT No effect on distance travelled, time in center or center entries Mooney-Leber and Brummelte (2020) RNP (4 NPs in left hind paw) TC, UC Rats P1–P7 P24, P45 and P66 Fear conditioning ↓ Auditory fear conditioning Davis et al. (2018) RNP (4 NPs, balanced over all paws) TC Rats P0–P7 Adult (P65) DWT

↑ latency to exit

↑ time in PVC tube

No effect on open field entries

Anand et al. (1999) aNP animals versus control animals. Abbreviations: DWT, defensive withdrawal testing; EPM, elevated plus maze; EZM, elevated zero maze; NPs, needle pricks; OFT, open field test; P, postnatal day; RNP, repetitive needle pricking; SD, Sprague-Dawley; TC, tactile and handled control; UC, undisturbed or unhandled controls.

The lack of concordance between tests for trait and state anxiety has previously been shown, where animals presenting high levels of anxiety in one test did not necessarily present the same levels in another test (Goes et al., 2009). The combination of neonatal repetitive pain with a second “hit” by adding the additional factors of height and unprotected areas in the EZM, may unmask the lower anxiety levels in NP animals as compared with the UCs in this test. It is important to highlight that although the combination of repetitive neonatal procedural pain and handling reduces state anxiety as compared with the undisturbed animals in our study, no differences with TC animals are observed. This suggests that the effect is driven by handling and/or removal as well as pain rather than pain alone, and the combination results in differential state anxiety levels. Daily postnatal handling during the first two postnatal weeks also reduced anxiety behavior in adulthood as compared with the UC (Li et al., 2018). In addition, differences in rearing conditions between UC and NP animals may also play a role (Reshetnikov et al., 2020). In preclinical studies, a second “hit” is often necessary for the long-term effects of neonatal pain to emerge (Williams & Lascelles, 2020). The impact of a subsequent injury in adulthood on anxiety level of rodents previously exposed to neonatal pain has not been evaluated yet. Contrary to preclinical findings showing lower state anxiety after repetitive needle pricking, the incidence of anxiety disorder is higher in children and adolescents born premature (Botting et al., 1997; Indredavik et al., 2004; Johnson et al., 2010; Walker et al., 2018). Patients with clinical anxiety tend to possess greater anxious traits in comparison with the healthy subjects, whereas state anxiety is not related to anxiety disorders (Kennedy et al., 2001). Hence, the effects of neonatal procedural pain may therefore not always emerge in clinical context. NICU admittance alone is already stressful, but the added neonatal pain can strengthen these effects. Overall, NICU infants appear to be surprisingly robust and adaptable; effects of neonatal pain exposure on later-life behavioral adversity are subtle rather than extreme, although the effects of pain remain difficult to discriminate from anxiety or fear in a clinical setting. In this context, follow-up studies should elucidate whether differences in trait and state anxiety exist in ex-preterm patients in later life. In conclusion, our study shows that in a rodent model of repetitive needle pricking, early-life procedural pain (including repetitive handling and removal) decreases state but not trait anxiety behavior in later life. On top of adequate analgesia, treatment of neonatal procedural pain should therefore take these adverse effects into account.

AUTHOR CONTRIBUTION

A. R. d. K., E. A. J., and N. v. d. H. designed and conceptualized the study and contributed to its structure and content. A. R. d. K. collected and analyzed the data. A. R. d. K. drafted the manuscript and figures. All authors commented on previous versions of the manuscript, critically revised, and quality assessed the manuscript. All authors have read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST

The authors have no conflict of interests to declare.

FUNDING

A. R. d. K. is financially supported by the Pain Knowledge Center from Maastricht and an institutional grant from University Maastricht, School Mental Health and Neuroscience.

留言 (0)

沒有登入
gif