Your action does matter to me: Examining the role of the co-actor’s action-effects in resolving the self-other discrimination problem

Baess, P., Prinz, W. (2015). My partner is also on my mind: Social context modulates the N1 response. Experimental Brain Research, 233(1), 105–113. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-014-4092-9
Google Scholar Coco, M. I., Dale, R., Keller, F. (2017). Performance in a collaborative search task: The role of feedback and alignment. Topics in Cognitive Science, 10(1), 55–79. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12300
Google Scholar Cook, R., Bird, G., Lünser, G., Huck, S., Heyes, C. (2012). Automatic imitation in a strategic context: Players of rock-paper-scissors imitate opponents’ gestures. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279(1729), 780–786. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2011.1024
Google Scholar Dittrich, K., Dolk, T., Rothe-Wulf, A., Klauer, K. C., Prinz, W. (2013). Keys and seats: Spatial response coding underlying the joint spatial compatibility effect. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75(8), 1725–1736. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-013-0524-z
Google Scholar Dolk, T., Hommel, B., Colzato, L. S., Schütz-Bosbach, S., Prinz, W., Liepelt, R. (2014). The joint Simon effect: A review and theoretical integration. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, Article 974. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00974
Google Scholar Dolk, T., Hommel, B., Prinz, W., Liepelt, R. (2013). The (not so) social Simon effect: A referential coding account. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39(5), 1248–1260. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031031
Google Scholar Dolk, T., Liepelt, R. (2018). The multimodal go-nogo Simon effect: Signifying the relevance of stimulus features in the go-nogo Simon paradigm impacts event representations and task performance. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, Article 2011. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02011
Google Scholar Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., Buchner, A. (2007). G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146
Google Scholar Forbes, P. A. G., Suddell, S. F., Farmer, H., Logeswaran, Y., Hamilton, A. F. d. C. (2019). The way others move can influence what we choose. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72(7), 1756–1770. https://doi.org/10.1177/1747021818808461
Google Scholar Glover, S., Dixon, P. (2017). The role of predictability in cooperative and competitive joint action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43(4), 644–650. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000362
Google Scholar Hommel, B. (1996). SR compatibility effects without response uncertainty. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A, 49, 546–571. https://doi.org/10.1080/713755643
Google Scholar Hommel, B. (2004). Event files: Feature binding in and across perception and action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(11), 494–500. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.08.007
Google Scholar Hommel, B. (2009). Action control according to TEC (theory of event coding). Psychological Research, 73, 512–526. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-009-0234-2
Google Scholar Hommel, B. (2018). Representing oneself and others: An event-coding approach. Experimental Psychology, 65, 323–331. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000433
Google Scholar Hommel, B. (2019). Theory of Event Coding (TEC) V2. 0: Representing and controlling perception and action. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 81(7), 2139–2154. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-019-01779-4
Google Scholar Keller, P. E., Novembre, G., Loehr, J. (2016). Musical ensemble performance: Representing self, other and joint action outcomes. In Obhi, S. S., Cross, E. S. (Eds.), Sensorimotor foundations of social life (pp. 280–310). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781107279353.015
Google Scholar Klempova, B., Liepelt, R. (2018). Barriers to success: Physical separation optimizes event-file retrieval in shared workspaces. Psychological Research, 82, 1158–1176. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0886-2
Google Scholar Knoblich, G., Sebanz, N. (2008). Evolving intentions for social interaction: From entrainment to joint action. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1499), 2021–2031. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0006
Google Scholar Kühn, S., Keizer, A. W., Colzato, L. S., Rombouts, S. A., Hommel, B. (2011). The neural underpinnings of event-file management: Evidence for stimulus-induced activation of and competition among stimulus–response bindings. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(4), 896–904. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2010.21485
Google Scholar Memelink, J., Hommel, B. (2013). Intentional weighting: A basic principle in cognitive control. Psychological Research, 77(3), 249–259. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-012-0435-y
Google Scholar Mussi, D. R., Marino, B. F., Riggio, L. (2015). The influence of social and nonsocial variables on the Simon effect. Experimental Psychology, 62, 215–231. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000292
Google Scholar Perugini, M., Gallucci, M., Costantini, G. (2018). A practical primer to power analysis for simple experimental designs. International Review of Social Psychology, 31(1). https://doi.org/10.5334/irsp.181
Google Scholar Pfister, R., Dolk, T., Prinz, W., Kunde, W. (2014). Joint response–effect compatibility. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(3), 817–822. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0528-7
Google Scholar Sacheli, L. M., Arcangeli, E., Paulesu, E. (2018). Evidence for a dyadic motor plan in joint action. Scientific Reports, 8, Article 5027. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-23275-9
Google Scholar Sacheli, L. M., Meyer, M., Hartstra, E., Bekkering, H., Hunnius, S. (2019). How preschoolers and adults represent their joint action partner’s behavior. Psychological Research, 83(5), 863–877. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0929-8
Google Scholar Schmitz, L., Vesper, C., Sebanz, N., Knoblich, G. (2017). Co-representation of others’ task constraints in joint action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43(8), 1480–1493. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000403
Google Scholar Sebanz, N., Knoblich, G., Prinz, W. (2003). Representing others’ actions: Just like one’s own? Cognition, 88(3), B11–B21. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00043-X
Google Scholar Sellaro, R., Dolk, T., Colzato, L. S., Liepelt, R., Hommel, B. (2015). Referential coding does not rely on location features: Evidence for a nonspatial joint Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41(1), 186–195. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038548
Google Scholar Simon, J. R. (1969). Reactions toward the source of stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81(1), 174–176. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0027448
Google Scholar To, M., Lovell, P. G., Troscianko, T., Tolhurst, D. J. (2008). Summation of perceptual cues in natural visual scenes. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 275(1649), 2299–2308. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2008.0692
Google Scholar Vesper, C., Schmitz, L., Knoblich, G. (2017). Modulating action duration to establish nonconventional communication. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 146(12), 1722–1737. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000379
Google Scholar Vesper, C., Schmitz, L., Safra, L., Sebanz, N., Knoblich, G. (2016). The role of shared visual information for joint action coordination. Cognition, 153, 118–123. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.05.002
Google Scholar Vesper, C., van der Wel, R. P., Knoblich, G., Sebanz, N. (2013). Are you ready to jump? Predictive mechanisms in interpersonal coordination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39(1), 48–61. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028066
Google Scholar Vlainic, E., Liepelt, R., Colzato, L. S., Prinz, W., Hommel, B. (2010). The virtual co-actor: The social Simon effect does not rely on online feedback from the other. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, Article 208. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00208
Google Scholar Welsh, T. N. (2009). When 1 + 1 = 1: The unification of independent actors revealed through joint Simon effects in crossed and uncrossed effector conditions. Human Movement Science, 28(6), 726–737. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2009.07.006
Google Scholar Yamaguchi, M., Clarke, E. L., Egan, D. L. (2018). Is your color my color? Dividing the labor of the Stroop task between co-actors. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, Article 1407. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01407
Google Scholar Yamaguchi, M., Proctor, R. W. (2012). Multidimensional vector model of stimulus–response compatibility. Psychological Review, 119(2), 272–303. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026620
Google Scholar

留言 (0)

沒有登入
gif