Intoxication without anticipation: Disentangling pharmacological from expected effects of alcohol

Band, GPH, van der Molen, MW, Logan, GD (2003) Horse-race model simulations of the stop-signal procedure. Acta Psychologica 112(2): 105–142.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Borsari, B, Carey, KB (2001) Peer influences on college drinking: A review of the research. Journal of Substance Abuse 13(4): 391–424.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Caswell, AJ, Morgan, MJ, Duka, T (2013) Acute alcohol effects on subtypes of impulsivity and the role of alcohol-outcome expectancies. Psychopharmacology 229(1): 21–30.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Christiansen, P, Jennings, E, Rose, AK (2016) Anticipated effects of alcohol stimulate craving and impair inhibitory control. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 30(3): 383–388.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Christiansen, P, Mansfield, R, Duckworth, J, et al. (2015) Internal reliability of the alcohol-related visual probe task is increased by utilising personalised stimuli and eye-tracking. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 155: 170–174.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Christiansen, P, Rose, AK, Cole, JC, et al. (2013) A comparison of the anticipated and pharmacological effects of alcohol on cognitive bias, executive function, craving and ad-lib drinking. Journal of Psychopharmacology 27(1): 84–92.
Google Scholar | SAGE Journals | ISI Christiansen, P, Townsend, G, Knibb, G, et al. (2017) Bibi ergo sum: The effects of a placebo and contextual alcohol cues on motivation to drink alcohol. Psychopharmacology 234(5): 827–835.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Davies, JB, Best, DW (1996) Demand characteristics and research into drug use. Psychology & Health 11(2): 291–299.
Google Scholar | Crossref de Wit, H (1996) Priming effects with drugs and other reinforcers. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology 4(1): 5–10.
Google Scholar | Crossref Duka, T, Townshend, JM (2004) The priming effect of alcohol pre-load on attentional bias to alcohol-related stimuli. Psychopharmacology 176(3–4): 353–362.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Fernie, G, Christiansen, P, Cole, JC, et al. (2012) Effects of 0.4g/kg alcohol on attentional bias and alcohol-seeking behaviour in heavy and moderate social drinkers. Journal of Psychopharmacology 26(7): 1017–1025.
Google Scholar | SAGE Journals | ISI Field, M, Christiansen, P (2012) Commentary on Ataya et al. (2012), Internal reliability of measures of substance-related cognitive bias. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 124(3): 189–190.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Field, M, Cox, WM (2008) Attentional bias in addictive behaviors: A review of its development, causes, and consequences. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 97(1–2): 1–20.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline | ISI Field, M, Jones, A (2017) Elevated alcohol consumption following alcohol cue exposure is partially mediated by reduced inhibitory control and increased craving. Psychopharmacology 234(19): 2979–2988.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Field, M, Wiers, RW, Christiansen, P, et al. (2010) Acute alcohol effects on inhibitory control and implicit cognition: Implications for loss of control over drinking. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 34(8): 1346–1352.
Google Scholar | Medline | ISI Fillmore, MT, Blackburn, J (2002) Compensating for alcohol-induced impairment: Alcohol expectancies and behavioral disinhibition. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 63(2): 237–246.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Fillmore, MT, Weafer, J (2013) Behavioral inhibition and addiction. In MacKillop, J, de Wit, H (eds) The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of addiction psychopharmacology. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 135–164.
Google Scholar | Crossref Franken, IHA (2003) Drug craving and addiction: Integrating psychological and neuropsychopharmacological approaches. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry 27(4): 563–579.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline | ISI Jones, A, Christiansen, P, Nederkoorn, C, et al. (2013) Fluctuating disinhibition: Implications for the understanding and treatment of alcohol and other substance use disorders. Frontiers in Psychiatry 4: 140.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Jones, A, Guerrieri, R, Fernie, G, et al. (2011) The effects of priming restrained versus disinhibited behaviour on alcohol-seeking in social drinkers. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 113(1): 55–61.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Jones, A, Tiplady, B, Houben, K, et al. (2018) Do daily fluctuations in inhibitory control predict alcohol consumption? An ecological momentary assessment study. Psychopharmacology 235(5): 1487–1496.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Karam, E, Kypri, K, Salamoun, M (2007) Alcohol use among college students: An international perspective. Current Opinion in Psychiatry 20(3): 213–221.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline | ISI Knight, JR, Wechsler, H, Kuo, M, et al. (2002) Alcohol abuse and dependence among U.S. college students. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 63(3): 263–270.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline | ISI Logan, GD, Cowan, WB, Davis, KA (1984) On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: A model and a method. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 10(2): 276–291.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline | ISI Logan, GD, Schachar, RJ, Tannock, R (1997) Impulsivity and inhibitory control. Psychological Science 8(1): 60–64.
Google Scholar | SAGE Journals | ISI Love, A, James, D, Willner, P (1998) A comparison of two alcohol craving questionnaires. Addiction 93(7): 1091–1102.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline MacAndrew, C, Edgerton, RB (1969) Drunken Comportment : A Social Explanation. Clinton Corners, NY: Percheron Press.
Google Scholar McNeill, A, Monk, RL, Qureshi, AW, et al. (2018) Continuous theta burst transcranial magnetic stimulation of the right Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex impairs inhibitory control and increases alcohol consumption. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience 18(6): 1198–1206.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline McNeill, AM, Monk, RL, Qureshi, AW, et al. (2021) The effects of placebo and moderate dose alcohol on attentional bias, inhibitory control and subjective craving. Alcohol and Alcoholism. Epub ahead of print 8 march. DOI: 10.1093/alcalc/agab011.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Marlatt, GA, Demming, B, Reid, JB (1973) Loss of control drinking in alcoholics: An experimental analogue. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 81(3): 233–241.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Monem, R, Fillmore, MT (2019) Alcohol administration reduces attentional bias to alcohol-related but not food-related cues: Evidence for a satiety hypothesis HHS public access. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 33(8): 677–684.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Monk, RL, Sunley, J, Qureshi, AW, et al. (2016) Smells like inhibition: The effects of olfactory and visual alcohol cues on inhibitory control. Psychopharmacology 233(8): 1331–1337.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Patton, JH, Stanford, MS, Barratt, ES (1995) Factor structure of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology 51(6): 768–774.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline | ISI Pennington, CR, Jones, A, Bartlett, JE, et al. (2021) Raising the bar: Improving methodological rigour in cognitive alcohol research. Addiction. Epub ahead of print 17 May. DOI: 10.1111/add.15563.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Rose, AK, Grunsell, L (2008) The subjective, rather than the disinhibiting, effects of alcohol are related to binge drinking. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 32(6): 1096–1104.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Rose, AK, Hobbs, M, Drummond, C (2013) Differentiating the contribution of pharmacological from alcohol expectancy effects to changes in subjective response and priming over successive drinks. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 37(4): 687–695.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Rose, AK, Jones, A, Clarke, N, et al. (2014) Alcohol-induced risk taking on the BART mediates alcohol priming. Psychopharmacology 231(11): 2273–2280.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Saunders, JB, Aasland, OG, Babor, TF, et al. (1993) Development of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption: II. Addiction 88(6): 791–804.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline | ISI Schoenmakers, T, Wiers, RW, Field, M (2008) Effects of a low dose of alcohol on cognitive biases and craving in heavy drinkers. Psychopharmacology 197(1): 169–178.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Sobell, LC, Sobell, MB (1990) Self-report issues in alcohol abuse: State of the art and future directions. Behavioral Assessment 12(1): 77–90.
Google Scholar Spanakis, P, Jones, A, Field, M, et al. (2019) A Stroop in the hand is worth two on the laptop: Superior reliability of a smartphone based alcohol Stroop in the real world. Substance Use and Misuse 54(4): 692–698.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Verbruggen, F, Logan, GD, Stevens, MA (2008) STOP-IT: Windows executable software for the stop-signal paradigm. Behavior Research Methods 40(2): 479–483.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline | ISI Volkow, ND, Koob, GF, McLellan, AT (2016) Neurobiologic advances from the brain disease model of addiction. New England Journal of Medicine 374(4): 363–371.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline | ISI Weafer, J, Fillmore, MT (2008) Individual differences in acute alcohol impairment of inhibitory control predict ad libitum alcohol consumption. Psychopharmacology 201(3): 315–324.
Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline Weafer, J, Fillmore, MT (2016) Low-dose alcohol effects on measures of inhibitory control, delay discounting, and risk-taking. Current Addiction Reports 3(1): 75–84.
Google Scholar | Crossref

留言 (0)

沒有登入
gif